Read The Flamingo’s Smile Online
Authors: Stephen Jay Gould
What evidence, then, do these cases provide for selection of sexual cannibalism among males? Do males, for the sake of their genetic continuity, actively elicit (or even passively submit to) the care and feeding of their fertilized eggs with their own bodies? I find little persuasive evidence for such a phenomenon in these cases, and I wonder if it exists at all—although the argument would provide an excellent illustration of a curiosity that makes little sense unless the evolutionary world works for reproductive success of individuals, as Darwinism argues.
The scorpion story, despite its citation among best cases, provides no evidence at all. As I read Polis and Farley, I note only that males try their best to escape after copulation and succeed in a great majority of cases (only two failed). Indeed, their mating behavior, both before and after, seems designed to avoid destruction, not to court it. Before, they turn off the female’s aggressive instincts by marching and stroking. After, they hit and run. That a few fail and get eaten reflects the inevitable odds of any dangerous game that must be played.
Black widow spiders and praying mantises offer more to the theory of direct selection for destruction among males. The spiders seem to be as cautious as scorpions before, but quite lackadaisical after, making little if any attempt to escape from the female’s web. In addition, if the mating plug that they leave in the female debars them from any future patrimony, then they have fully served their Darwinian purpose. As for mantises, the better performance of a headless male might indicate that sex and death have been actively conjoined by selection. Yet, in both these cases, other observations render more than a bit ambiguous any evidence for active selection on males.
As a major problem for both mantises and spiders, we have no good evidence about the frequency of sexual cannibalism. If it occurred always or even often and if the male clearly stopped and just let it happen, then I would be satisfied that this reasonable phenomenon exists. But if it occurs rarely and represents a simple failure to escape, rather than an active offering, then it is a byproduct of other phenomena, not a selected trait in itself. I can find no quantitative data on the percentage of eating after mating either in nature or even in the more unsatisfactory and artificial conditions of a laboratory.
For mantises, I find no evidence for the male’s complicity in his demise. Males are cautious beforehand and zealous to escape thereafter. But the female is big and rapacious; she makes no distinction between a smaller mantis and any other moving prey. As for the curious fact of better performance in decapitated males, I simply don’t know. It could be a direct adaptation for combining sex with consumption, but other interpretations fare just as well in our absence of evidence. Hard-wired behavior must be programmed in some way. Perhaps the system of inhibition by a ganglion in the head and activation by one near the tail evolved in an ancestral lineage long before sexual cannibalism ever arose among mantises. Perhaps it was already in place when female mantises evolved their indiscriminate rapacity. It would then be co-opted, not actively selected, for its useful role in sexual cannibalism. After all, the same system works for females too, although their behavior serves no known evolutionary function. Decapitate a female mantis and you also unleash sexual behavior, including egg laying. If one wishes to argue that the system must have been actively evolved because the female tends to eat first just that portion of the male that unleashes sexuality, I reply with a bit of biology at its most basic: heads are in front and females encounter them first as the male approaches.
The black widow story is also shaky. Males may not try to escape after mating, but is this an active adaptation for consumption or an automatic response to the real adaptation—breaking of the sexual organ and deposition of a mating plug in the female (for such an injury might weaken the male and explain his subsequent lassitude)? Also, male black widows are tiny compared with their mates—only 2 percent or so of the female’s weight. Will such a small meal make enough of a difference? Finally, and most importantly, how often does the female partake of this available meal? If she always ate the exhausted male after mating, I would be more persuaded. But some studies indicate that sexual cannibalism may be rare, even though clearly available as an option for females. Curiously, several articles report that males often stay on the female’s web until they die, often for two weeks or more, and that females leave them alone. Ross and Smith, for example, noticed only one case of sexual cannibalism and wrote: “Only one male of those we observed to succeed in inseminating a female was eaten by its mate immediately after mating. However, several were later found dead in their mates’ webs.”
Why then, in this disturbing absence of evidence, does our literature abound with comments on the obvious evolutionary good sense of sexual cannibalism? For example: “Under some conditions selection should favor the consumption of males by their mates. His probability of being cannibalized should be directly proportional to the male’s future expectation of reproduction.” Or, “Successful males would best serve their biological interests by presenting themselves to their mates as a post-nuptial meal.”
In this hiatus between reasonable hope and actual evidence, we come face to face with a common bias of modern Darwinism. Darwinian theory is fundamentally about natural selection. I do not challenge this emphasis, but believe that we have become overzealous about the power and range of selection by trying to attribute every significant form and behavior to its direct action. In this Darwinian game, no prize is sweeter than a successful selectionist interpretation for phenomena that strike our intuition as senseless. How could a male become a blood meal after mating if selection rules our world? Because, in certain situations, he increases his own reproductive success thereby, our devoted selectionist responds.
But another overarching, yet often forgotten, evolutionary principle usually intervenes and prevents any optimal match between organism and immediate environment—the curious, tortuous, constraining pathways of history. Organisms are not putty before a molding environment or billiard balls before the pool cue of natural selection. Their inherited forms and behaviors constrain and push back; they cannot be quickly transformed to new optimality every time the environment alters.
Every adaptive change brings scores of consequences in its wake, some luckily co-opted for later advantage, others not. Some large females evolve indiscriminate rapacity for their own reasons, and some males suffer the consequences despite their own evolutionary race to escape. Designs evolved for one reason (or no reason) have other consequences, some fortuitously useful. Male mantises can become headless wonders; male black widows remain on the female’s web. Both behaviors may be useful, but we have no evidence that either arose by active selection for male sacrifice. Sexual cannibalism with active male complicity should be favored in many groups (for the conditions of limited opportunity after mating and useful fodder are often met), but it has evolved rarely, if ever. Ask why we don’t see it where it should occur; don’t simply marvel about the wisdom of selection in a few possible cases. History often precludes useful opportunity; you cannot always get there from here. Females may not be sufficiently rapacious, or they may be smaller than males, or so limited in behavioral flexibility that they cannot evolve a system to turn off a general inhibition against cannibalism only after mating and only toward a male.