Authors: Daniel Bor
D. J. Simons and D. T. Levin, Failure to detect changes to people during a real-world interaction.
Psychon Bull Rev
, 1998. 5(4): 644–649.
D. J. Simons and C. F. Chabris, Gorillas in our midst: sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events.
Perception
, 1999. 28(9): 1059–1074.
R. Desimone and J. Duncan, Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention.
Annu Rev Neurosci
, 1995. 18: 193–222.J. W. Bisley and M. E. Goldberg, Neuronal activity in the lateral intraparietal area and spatial attention.
Science
, 2003. 299(5603): 81–86.M. Carrasco, C. Penpeci-Talgar, and M. Eckstein, Spatial covert attention increases contrast sensitivity across the CSF: support for signal enhancement.
Vision Res
, 2000. 40(10–12): 1203–1215.
M. Carrasco, S. Ling, and S. Read, Attention alters appearance.
Nat Neurosci
, 2004. 7(3): 308–313.
Desimone and Duncan, Neural mechanisms.J. Duncan, EPS Mid-Career Award 2004: brain mechanisms of attention.
Q J Exp Psychol
(Colchester), 2006. 59(1): 2–27.
R. P. Feynman, R. B. Leighton, and M. Sands,
Lectures on physics: complete set
, vols. 1–3. 1998, Boston: Addison Wesley.
J. Sharma, A. Angelucci, and M. Sur, Induction of visual orientation modules in auditory cortex.
Nature
, 2000. 404(6780): 841–847.L. Von Melchner, S. L. Pallas, and M. Sur, Visual behaviour mediated by retinal projections directed to the auditory pathway.
Nature
, 2000. 404(6780): 871–876.H. Burton et al., Adaptive changes in early and late blind: a fMRI study of Braille reading.
J Neurophysiol
, 2002. 87(1): 589–607.
Desimone and Duncan (1995), see above.Duncan (2006), see above.E. I. Knudsen, Fundamental components of attention.
Annu Rev Neurosci
, 2007. 30: 57–78.
A. R. Damasio,
Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain
. 1994, New York: HarperCollins.
E. Bliss,
Multiple personality, allied disorders, and hypnosis
. 1986, Oxford: Oxford University Press.
H. Lau and D. Rosenthal, Empirical support for higher-order theories of conscious awareness.
Trends Cogn Sci
, 2011. 15(8): 365–373.
S. M. Fleming et al., Relating introspective accuracy to individual differences in brain structure.
Science
, 2010. 329(5998): 1541–1543.
N. Cowan, The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity.
Behav Brain Sci
, 2001. 24(1): 87–114; discussion 114–185.
B. J. Baars,
In the theater of consciousness: the workspace of the mind
. 1997, New York: Oxford University Press.B. J. Baars and S. Franklin, An architectural model of conscious and unconscious brain functions: Global Workspace Theory and IDA.
Neural Netw
, 2007. 20(9): 955–961.
G. Sperling, The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychological Monographs, 1960. 74(11, Whole No. 498): 1–29.
S. Yantis, Multielement visual tracking: attention and perceptual organization.
Cogn Psychol
, 1992. 24(3): 295–340.
E. Heyselaar, K. Johnston, and M. Paré, The capacity limit of the visual working memory of the macaque monkey.
J Vision
. 10(7): 725.
H. J. Gross et al., Number-based visual generalisation in the honeybee. PLoS One, 2009. 4(1): e4263.
A. Raffone and G. Wolters, A cortical mechanism for binding in visual working memory.
J Cogn Neurosci
, 2001. 13(6): 766–785.
J. Duncan, Similarity between concurrent visual discriminations: dimensions and objects.
Percept Psychophys
, 1993. 54(4): 425–430.
K. A. Ericcson, W. G. Chase, and S. Falloon, Acquisition of a memory skill.
Science
, 1980. 208: 1181–1182.
D. Bor and A. M. Owen, A common prefrontal-parietal network for mnemonic and mathematical recoding strategies within working memory.
Cereb Cortex
, 2007. 17(4): 778–786.
W. G. Chase and H. A. Simon, Perception in chess.
Cogn Psychol
, 1973. 4: 55–81.
G. A. Miller, The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information.
Psychol Rev
, 1956. 63(2): 81–97.
C. Bundesen, H. Shibuya, and A. Larsen, Visual selection from multielement displays: a model for partial report, in
Attention and performance XI
, M. I. Posner and M.S.I. Marin, eds. 1985, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 631–649.J. J. Todd and R. Marois, Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex.
Nature
, 2004. 428(6984): 751–754.
C. Bundesen, T. Habekost, and S. Kyllingsbaek, A neural theory of visual attention: bridging cognition and neurophysiology.
Psychol Rev
, 2005. 112(2): 291–328.
L. Chelazzi et al., Responses of neurons in inferior temporal cortex during memory-guided visual search.
J Neurophysiol
, 1998. 80(6): 2918–2940.
M. A. Cohen, G. A. Alvarez, and K. Nakayama, Natural-scene perception requires attention.
Psychol Sci
, 2011. 22(9): 1165–1172.
J. S. Joseph, M. M. Chun, and K. Nakayama, Attentional requirements in a “preattentive” feature search task.
Nature
, 1997. 387(6635): 805–807.J. Theeuwes, A. F. Kramer, and P. Atchley, Attentional effects on preattentive vision: spatial precues affect the detection of simple features.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform
, 1999. 25(2): 341–347.S. Walker, P. Stafford, and G. Davis, Ultra-rapid categorization requires visual attention: scenes with multiple foreground objects.
J Vision
, 2008. 8(4): 211–212.
Cowan (2001), see above.
Miller (1956), see above.
D. Hofstadter,
Godel, Escher, Bach: an eternal golden braid (20th anniversary edition, with a new preface by the author)
. 1999, London: Penguin.
L. Melloni et al., Expectations change the signatures and timing of electrophysiological correlates of perceptual awareness.
J Neurosci
, 2011. 31(4): 1386–1396.C. Summerfield and T. Egner, Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition.
Trends Cogn Sci
, 2009. 13(9): 403–409.
S. Savage-Rumbaugh and R. Lewin,
Kanzi: the ape at the brink of the human mind
. 1994, New York: John Wiley & Sons.
S. Pinker,
The language instinct: the new science of language and mind
. 2003, London: Penguin.
D. Bor et al., Encoding strategies dissociate prefrontal activity from working memory demand.
Neuron
, 2003. 37(2): 361–367.P. Fletcher et al., Learning-related neuronal responses in prefrontal cortex studied with functional neuroimaging.
Cereb Cortex
, 1999. 9(2): 168–178.
F. Vargha-Khadem et al., FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language
. Nat Rev Neurosci
, 2005. 6(2): 131–138.
D. Bor and A. K. Seth, Consciousness and the prefrontal parietal network: insights from attention, working memory and chunking.
Front Psychol,
2012. 3: 63.
S. L. Beilock et al., Haste does not always make waste: expertise, direction of attention, and speed versus accuracy in performing sensorimotor skills.
Psychon Bull Rev
, 2004. 11(2): 373–379.S. L. Beilock et al., When paying attention becomes counterproductive: impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills.
J Exp Psychol Appl
, 2002. 8(1): 6–16.