Read The Oxford History of World Cinema Online
Authors: Geoffrey Nowell-Smith
also the forerunners of electronic image-making. The first experiments in transmitting
images by a television-type device are in fact as old as the cinema: Adriano de Paiva
published his first studies on the subject in 1880, and Georges Rignoux seems to have
achieved an actual transmission in 1909. Meanwhile certain 'pre-cinema' techniques
continued to be used in conjunction with cinema proper during the years around 1900-5
when the cinema was establishing itself as a new mass medium of entertainment and
instruction, and lantern slides with movement effects continued for a long time to be
shown in close conjunction with film screenings.
Magic lantern, film, and television, therefore, do not constitute three separate universes
(and fields of study), but belong together as part of a single process of evolution. It is
none the less possible to distinguish them, not only technologically and in terms of the
way they were diffused, but also chronologically. The magic lantern show gradually gives
way to the film show at the beginning of the twentieth century, while television emerges
fully only in the second half of the century. In this succession, what distinguishes cinema
is on the one hand its technological base -- photographic images projected in quick
succession giving the illusion of continuity -- and on the other hand its use prevailingly as
large-scale public entertainment.
THE BASIC APPARATUS
Films produce their illusion of continuous movement by passing a series of discrete
images in quick succession in front of a light source enabling the images to be projected
on a screen. Each image is held briefly in front of the light and then rapidly replaced with
the next one. If the procedure is rapid and smooth enough, and the images similar enough
to each other, discontinuous images are then perceived as continuous and an illusion of
movement is created. The perceptual process involved was known about in the nineteenth
century, and given the name persistence of vision, since the explanation was thought to lie
in the persistence of the image on the retina of the eye for long enough to make
perception of each image merge into the perception of the next one. This explanation is no
longer regarded as adequate, and modern psychology prefers to see the question in terms
of brain functions rather than of the eye alone. But the original hypothesis was
sufficiently fertile to lead to a number of experiments in the 1880s and 1890s aimed at
reproducing the so-called persistence of vision effect with sequential photographs.
The purposes of these experiments were various. They were both scientific and
commercial, aimed at analysing movement and at reproducing it. In terms of the
emergence of cinema the most important were those which set out to reproduce
movement naturally, by taking pictures at a certain speed (a minimum of ten or twelve per
second and generally higher) and showing them at the same speed. In fact throughout the
silent period the correspondence between camera speed and projection was rarely perfect.
A projection norm of around 16 pictures ('frames') per second seems to have been the
most common well into the 1920s, but practices differed considerably and it was always
possible for camera speeds to be made deliberately slower or faster to produce effects of
speeded-up or slowed-down motion when the film was projected. It was only with the
coming of synchronized sound-tracks, which had to be played at a constant speed, that a
norm of 24 frames per second (f.p.s.) became standard for both camera and projector.
First of all, however, a mechanism had to be created which would enable the pictures to
be exposed in the camera in quick succession and projected the same way. A roll of
photographic film had to be placed in the camera and alternately held very still while the
picture was exposed and moved down very fast to get on to the next picture, and the same
sequence had to be followed when the film was shown. Moving the film and then
stopping it so frequently put considerable strain on the film itself -- a problem which was
more severe in the projector than in the camera, since the negative was exposed only once
whereas the print would be shown repeatedly. The problem of intermittent motion, as it is
called, exercised the minds of many of the pioneers of cinema, and was solved only by
the introduction of a small loop in the threading of the film where it passed the gate in
front of the lens (see inset).
FILM STOCK
The moving image as a form of collective entertainment -what we call 'cinema' --
developed and spread in the form of photographic images printed on a flexible and
semitransparent celluloid base, cut into strips 35 mm. wide. This material -- 'film' -- was
devised by Henry M. Reichenbach for George Eastman in 1889, on the basis of inventions
variously attributed to the brothers J. W. and I. S. Hyatt ( 1865), to Hannibal Goodwin
( 1888), and to Reichenbach himself. The basic components of the photographic film used
since the end of the nineteenth century have remained unchanged over the years. They
are: a transparent
base,
or
support;
a very fine layer of
adhesive substrate
made of
gelatine; and a light-sensitive
emulsion
which makes the film opaque on one side. The
emulsion generally consists of a suspension of silver salts in gelatine and is attached to
the base by means of the layer of adhesive substrate. The base of the great majority of 35
mm. films produced before February 1951 consists of
cellulose nitrate,
which is a highly
flammable substance. From that date onwards the nitrate base has been replaced by one of
cellulose acetate,
which is far less flammable, or increasingly by
polyester.
From early
times, however, various forms of 'safety' film were tried out, at first using cellulose
diacetate (invented by Eichengrun and Becker as early as 1901), or by coating the nitrate
in non-flammable substances. The first known examples of these procedures date back to
1909. Safety film became the norm for non-professional use after the First World War.
The black and white negative film used up to the mid1920s was so-called orthochromatic.
It was sensitive to ultraviolet, violet, and blue light, and rather less sensitive to green and
yellow. Red light did not affect the silver bromide emulsion at all. To prevent parts of the
scene from appearing on the screen only in the form of indistinct dark blobs, early
cinematographers had to practise a constant control of colour values on the set. Certain
colours had to be removed entirely from sets and costumes. Actresses avoided red
lipstick, and interior scenes were shot against sets painted in various shades of grey. A
new kind of emulsion called
panchromatic
was devised for Gaumont by the Eastman
Kodak Company in 1912. In just over a decade it became the preferred stock for all the
major production companies. It was less light-sensitive in absolute terms than
orthochrome, which meant that enhanced systems of studio lighting had to be developed.
But it was far better balanced and allowed for the reproduction of a wider range of greys.
In the early days, however, celluloid film was not the only material tried out in the
showing of motion pictures. Of alternative methods the best known was the Mutoscope.
This consisted of a cylinder to which were attached several hundred paper rectangles
about 70 mm. wide. These paper rectangles contained photographs which, if watched in
rapid sequence through a viewer, gave the impression of continuous movement. There
were even attempts to produce films on glass: the Kammatograph ( 1901) used a disc with
a diameter of 30 cm., containing some 600 photographic frames arranged in a spiral.
There were experiments involving the use of translucent metal with a photographic
emulsion on it which could be projected by reflection, and films with a surface in relief
which could be passed under the fingers of blind people, on a principle similar to Braille.
FORMATS
The 35 mm. width (or 'gauge') for cellulose was first adopted in 1892 by Thomas Edison
for his Kinetoscope, a viewing device which enabled one spectator at a time to watch
brief segments of film. The Kinetoscope was such a commercial success that subsequent
machines for reproducing images in movement adopted 35 mm. as a standard format.
This practice had the support of the Eastman Company, whose photographic film was 70
mm. wide, and therefore only had to be cut lengthwise to produce film of the required
width. It is also due to the mechanical structure of the Kinetoscope that 35 mm. film has
four perforations, roughly rectangular in shape, on both sides of each frame, used for
drawing the film through the camera and projector. Other pioneers at the end of the
nineteenth century used a different pattern. The Lumière brothers, for example, used a
single circular perforation on each side. But it was the Edison method which was soon
adopted as standard, and remains so today. It was the Edison company too who set the
standard size and shape of the 35 mm. frame, at approximately 1 in. wide and 0.75 in.
high.
Although these were to become the standards, there were many experiments with other
gauges of film stock, both in the early period and later. In 1896 the Prestwich Company
produced a 60 mm. film strip, an example of which is preserved in the National Film and
Television Archive in London, and the same width (but with a different pattern of
perforations) was used by Georges Demený in France. The Veriscope Company in
America introduced a 63 mm. gauge; one film in this format still survives -- a record of
the historic heavyweight championship fight between Corbett and Fitzsimmons in 1897.
Around the same time Louis Lumière also experimented with 70 mm. film which yielded
a picture area 60 mm. wide and 45 mm. high. All these systems encountered technical
problems, particularly in projection. Though some further experiments took place towards
the end of the silent period, the use of wide gauges such as 65 and 70 mm. did not come
into its own until the late 1950s.
More important than any attempts to expand the image, however, were those aimed at
reducing it and producing equipment suitable for non-professional users.
In 1900 the French company Gaumont began marketing its 'Chrono de Poche', a portable
camera which used 15 mm. film with a single perforation in the centre. Two years later
the Warwick Trading Company in England introduced a 17.5 mm. film for amateurs,
designed to be used on a machine called the Biokam which (like the first Lumière
machines) doubled as camera, printer, and projector; this idea was taken up by Ernemann
in Germany and then by Pathé in France in the 1920s. Meanwhile in 1912 Pathé had also
introduced a system that used 28 mm. film on a non-flammable diacetate base and had a
picture area only slightly smaller than 35 mm.
An alternative to celluloid film, the Kammatograph (c. 1900) used a glass disc with the film frames arranged in a spiral
The amateur gauge
par excellence,
however, was 16 mm. on a non-flammable base,
devised by Eastman Kodak in 1920. In its original version, known as the Kodascope, this
worked on the reversal principle, producing a direct positive print on the original film