The Lying Stones of Marrakech (53 page)

Read The Lying Stones of Marrakech Online

Authors: Stephen Jay Gould

BOOK: The Lying Stones of Marrakech
6.33Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub

In studying both kinds of populations, Reznick and colleagues found that “guppies from high-predation sites experience significantly higher mortality rates than those from low-predation sites.” They then reared both kinds of guppies under uniform conditions in the laboratory, and found that fishes from high-predation sites in lower drainages matured earlier and at a smaller size. “They also devote more resources to each litter, produce more, smaller offspring per litter, and produce litters more frequently than guppies from low-predation localities.”

This combination of observations from nature and the laboratory yields two important inferences. First, the differences make adaptive sense, for guppies subjected to greater predation would fare better if they could grow up fast and reproduce both copiously and quickly before the potential boom falls—a piscine equivalent of the old motto for electoral politics in Boston: vote early and vote often. On the other hand, guppies in little danger of being eaten might do better to bide their time and grow big and strong before engaging their fellows in any reproductive competition. Second, since these differences persist when both kinds of guppies are reared in identical laboratory environments, the distinction must record genetically based and inherited results of divergent evolution between the populations.

In 1981, Reznick had transferred some guppies from high-predation downstream pools into low-predation upstream waters then devoid of guppies. These transplanted populations evolved rapidly to adopt the reproductive strategy favored by indigenous populations in neighboring upstream environments: delayed sexual maturity at larger size, and longer life. Moreover, Reznick and colleagues made the interesting observation that males evolved considerably more rapidly in this favored direction. In one experiment, males reached their full extent of change within four years, while females continued to alter after eleven years. Since the laboratory populations had shown higher heritability for these traits in males than in females, these results make good sense. (Heritability may be roughly defined as the correlation between traits in parents and offspring due to genetic differences. The greater the heritable basis of a trait, the faster the feature can evolve by natural selection.)

This favorable set of circumstances—rapid evolution in a predictable and presumably adaptive direction based on traits known to be highly heritable—provides a “tight” case for well-documented (and sensible) evolution at scales
well within the purview of human observation, a mere decade in this case. The headline for the news report on this paper in
Science
magazine (March 28, 1997) read: “Predator-free guppies take an evolutionary leap forward.”

2. Lizards from the Exuma Cays, Bahama Islands. During most of my career, my fieldwork has centered on biology and paleontology of the land snail
Cerion
in the Bahama Islands. During these trips, I have often encountered fellow biologists devoted to other creatures. In one major program of research, Tom Schoener (a biology professor at the University of California, Davis) has, with numerous students and colleagues, been studying the biogeography and evolution of the ubiquitous little lizard
Anolis
—for me just a fleeting shadow running across a snail-studded ground, but for them a focus of utmost fascination (while my beloved snails, I assume, just blend into their immobile background).

In 1977 and 1981, Schoener and colleagues transplanted groups of five or ten lizards from Staniel Cay in the Exuma chain to fourteen small and neighboring islands that housed no lizards. In 1991, they found that the lizards had thrived (or at least survived and bred) on most of these islands, and they collected samples of adult males from each experimental island with an adequate population. In addition, they gathered a larger sample of males from areas on Staniel Cay that had served as the source for original transplantation in 1977 and 1981.

This study then benefits from general principles learned by extensive research on numerous
Anolis
species throughout the Bahama Islands. In particular, relatively longer limbs permit greater speed, a substantial advantage provided that preferred perching places can accommodate long-legged lizards. Trees and other “thick” perching places therefore favor the evolution of long legs. Staniel Cay itself includes a predominant forest, and the local
Anolis
tend to be long-legged. But when lizards must live on thin twigs in bushy vegetation, the agility provided by shorter legs (on such precarious perches) may outweigh the advantages in speed that longer legs would provide. Thus, lizards living on narrow twigs tend to be shorter-legged. The small cays that received the fourteen transported populations have little or no forest growth and tend instead to be covered with bushy vegetation (and narrow twigs).

J. B. Losos, the principal author of the new study, therefore based an obvious prediction on these generalities. The populations had been transferred from forests with wide perches to bushy islands covered with narrow twigs. “From the kind of vegetation on the new islands,” Losos stated, “we predicted that the lizards would develop shorter hindlimbs.” Their published study validates this
expected result: a clearly measurable change, in the predicted and adaptive direction, in less than twenty years. (See details in J. B. Losos, K. I. Warheit, and T. W. Schoener, “Adaptive differentiation following experimental island colonization in
Anolis
lizards,”
Nature
, 1997, volume 387, pages 70–73). A news report appeared in
Science
magazine (May 2, 1997) under the title “Catching lizards in the act of adapting.”

This study lacks a crucial piece of documentation that the Trinidadian guppies provided—an absence immediately noted by friendly critics and fully acknowledged by the authors. Losos and colleagues have not studied the heritability of leg length in
Anolis sagrei
and therefore cannot be certain that their results record a genetic process of evolutionary change. The growth of these lizards may feature extensive flexibility in leg length, so that the same genes yield longer legs if lizards grow up on trees, and shorter legs if they always cavort in the bushes (just as the same genes can lead to a thin or fat human being depending upon a personal history of nutrition and exercise). In any case, however, a sensible and apparently adaptive change in average leg length has occurred within twenty years on several islands, whatever the cause of modification.

3. Snails from Great Inagua, Bahama Islands. Most of Great Inagua, the second-largest Bahamian Island (Andros wins first prize), houses a large and ribby
Cerion
species named
C. rubicundum
. But fossil deposits of no great age lack this species entirely and feature instead an extinct form named
Cerion excelsior
, the largest of all
Cerion
species. Several years ago, on a mudflat in the southeastern corner of Great Inagua, David Woodruff (of the University of California, San Diego) and I collected a remarkable series of shells that seemed to span (and quite smoothly) the entire range of form from extinct
C. excelsior
to modern
C. rubicundum
. Moreover, and in general, the more eroded and “older looking” the shell, the closer it seemed to lie to the anatomy of extinct
C. excelsior
.

This situation suggested a local evolutionary transition by hybridization, as
C. rubicundum
, arriving on the island from an outside source, interbred with indigenous
C. excelsior
. Then, as
C. excelsior
declined toward extinction, while
C. rubicundum
thrived and increased, the average anatomy of the population transformed slowly and steadily in the direction of the modern form. This hypothesis sounded good and sensible, but we could devise no way to test our idea—for all the shells had been collected from a single mudflat (analogous to a single bedding plane of a geological stratum), and we could not determine their relative ages. The pure
C. excelsior
shells “looked” older, but such personal
impressions count for less than nothing (subject as they are to a researcher's bias) in science. So we got stymied and put the specimens in a drawer.

Several years later, I teamed up with paleontologist and geochemist Glenn A. Goodfriend from the Carnegie Institution of Washington. He had refined a dating technique based on changes in the composition of amino acids in the shell over time. By keying these amino acid changes to radiocarbon dates for some of the shells, we could estimate the age of each shell. A plot of shell age versus position on an anatomical spectrum from extinct
C. excelsior
to modern
C. rubicundum
produced a beautiful correlation between age and anatomy: the younger the specimen, the closer to the modern anatomy.

This ten- to twenty-thousand-year transition by hybridization exceeds the time period of the Trinidad and Exuma studies by three orders of magnitude (that is, by a factor of 1,000), but even ten thousand years represents a geological eye-blink in the fullness of evolutionary time—while this transformation in our snails marks a full change from one species to another, not just a small decrement of leg length, or a change in the timing of breeding, within a single species. (For details, see G. A. Goodfriend and S. J. Gould, “Paleontology and chronology of two evolutionary transitions by hybridization in the Bahamian land snail
Cerion
,”
Science
, 1996, volume 274, pages 1894–97). Harvard University's press release (with no input from me) carried the headline “Snails caught in act of evolving.”

A scanning of any year's technical literature in evolutionary biology would yield numerous and well-documented cases of such measurable, small-scale evolutionary change—thus disproving the urban legend that evolution must always be too slow to observe in the geological microsecond of a human lifetime. These three studies, all unusually complete in their documentation and in their resolution of details, do not really rank as “news” in the journalist's prime sense of novelty or deep surprise. Nonetheless, each of these three studies became subjects for front-page stories in either
The New York Times
or
The Boston Globe
.

Now please don't get me wrong. I do not belong to the cadre of rarefied academics who cringe at every journalistic story about science for fear that the work reported might become tainted with popularity thereby. And in a purely “political” sense, I certainly won't object if major newspapers choose to feature any result of my profession as a lead story—especially, if I may be self-serving for a moment, when one of the tales reports my own work! Nonetheless, this degree of public attention for workaday results in my field (however elegantly done) does fill me with wry amusement—if only for the general reason that
most of us feel a tickle in the funny bone when we note a gross imbalance between public notoriety and the true novelty or importance of an event, as when Hollywood spinmeisters manage to depict their client's ninth marriage as the earth's first example of true love triumphant and permanent.

Of course I'm delighted that some ordinary, albeit particularly well done studies of small-scale evolution struck journalists as front-page news. But I still feel impelled to ask why these studies, rather than a hundred others of equal care and merit that appear in our literature every month, caught this journalistic fancy and inspired such prime attention. When I muse over this issue, I can only devise two reasons—both based on deep and interesting fallacies well worth identifying and discussing. In this sense, the miselevation of everyday good work to surprising novelty may teach us something important about public attitudes toward evolution, and toward science in general. We may, I think, resolve each of the two fallacies by contrasting the supposed meaning of these studies, as reported in public accounts, with the significance of such work as viewed by professionals in the field.

1.
The fallacy of the crucial experiment

In high school physics classes, we all learned a heroically simplified version of scientific progress based upon a model that does work sometimes but by no means always—the
experimentum crucis
, or crucial experiment. Newton or Einstein? Ptolemy or Copernicus? Special Creation or Darwin? To find out, perform a single decisive experiment with a clearly measurable result replete with decisive power to decree yea or nay.

The decision to treat a limited and particular case as front-page news must be rooted in this fallacy. Reporters must imagine that evolution can be proved by a single crucial case, so that any of these stories may provide decisive confirmation of Darwin's truth—a matter of some importance given the urban legend that evolution, even if valid, must be invisible on human timescales.

But two counterarguments vitiate this premise. First, as a scientific or intellectual issue, we hardly need to “prove” evolution by discovering new and elegant cases. We do not, after all, expect to encounter a page-one story with the headline “New experiment proves earth goes around sun, not vice versa. Galileo vindicated.” The fact of evolution has been equally well documented for more than a century.

Second, and more generally, single “crucial” experiments rarely decide major issues in science—especially in natural history, where nearly all theories require data about “relative frequencies” (or percentage of occurrences), not
pristine single cases. Of course, for a person who believes that evolution never occurs at all, one good case can pack enormous punch—but science resolved this basic issue more than one hundred years ago. Nearly every interesting question in evolutionary theory asks “how often?” or “how dominant in setting the pattern of life?”—not “does this phenomenon occur at all?” For example, on the most important issue of all—the role of Darwin's own favored mechanism of natural selection—single examples of selection's efficacy advance the argument very little. We already know, by abundant documentation and rigorous theorizing, that natural selection can and does operate in nature. We need to determine the
relative strength
of Darwin's mechanism among a set of alternative modes for evolutionary change—and single cases, however elegant, cannot establish a relative frequency.

Other books

Dark Awakening by Charlotte Featherstone
Heart of the Hill by Andrea Spalding
The Mission Song by John le Carre
The Black Chronicle by Oldrich Stibor
Sticks by Joan Bauer
Genesis (The Exodus Trilogy) by Andreas Christensen
Stolen Fate by Linsey Hall