Read It's a Jungle in There: How Competition and Cooperation in the Brain Shape the Mind Online
Authors: David A. Rosenbaum
Johnson, A., & Proctor, R. W. (2003).
Attention: Theory and practice
. Los Angeles, CA: Sage.
Blumberg, M. S., & Wasserman, E. A. (1995). Animal mind and the argument from design.
American Psychologist, 50
, 133–144.
Johnson, G. (2008).
The ten most beautiful experiments
. New York, NY: Alfred A. Knopf.
Johnson, S. (2010).
Where good ideas come from: The natural history of innovation
. New York, NY: Riverhead Books.
Jonides, J., & Mack, R. (1984). On the cost and benefit of cost and benefit.
Psychological Bulletin, 96
, 29–44.
Jost, A. (1897). Die Assoziationsfestigkeit in ihrer Abha¨ngigkeit von der Verteilung der Wiederholungen [The strength of associations in their dependence on the distribution of repetitions].
Zeitschrift fur Psychologie und Physiologie der Sinnesorgane, 16
, 436–472.
Kahneman, D. (1973).
Attention and effort
. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Kahneman, D. (2011).
Thinking, fast and slow
. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Kamin, L. J. (1969). Predictability, surprise, attention and conditioning. In B. A. Campbell & R. M. Church (Eds.),
Punishment and aversive behavior
(pp. 279–96). New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H., & Jessell, T. M. (Eds.) (2000).
Principles of neural science
(4th ed.). New York, NY: McGraw Hill.
Keil, F. C. (1979).
Semantic and conceptual development: An ontological perspective
. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.
Keil, F. C. (1981). Constraints on knowledge and cognitive development.
Psychological Review, 88
, 197–227.
Kiesel, A., Stenhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A., & Koch, I. (2010). Control and interference in task switching—A review.
Psychological Bulletin, 136
, 849–874.
Klein, R. M. (2000). Inhibition of return.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4
, 138–147.
Kleinschmidt, A., Büchel, C., Zeki, S., & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1998). Human brain activity during spontaneously reversing perception of ambiguous figures.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 265
(1413), 2427–2433.
Kolers, P. A. (1976). Reading a year later.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2
, 554–565.
Konner, M. (2010).
The evolution of childhood: Relationships, emotion, mind
. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Körding, K. P., & Wolpert, D. M. (2004). Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning.
Nature, 437
, 244–247.
Koriat, A. (2012). When are two heads better than one and why?
Science, 336
, 360–362.
Kornblum, S. (1969). Sequential determinants of information processing in serial and discrete choice reaction time.
Psychological Review, 76
, 113–131.
Kroll, J. F., Michael, E., & Sankaranarayanan, A. (1998). A model of bilingual representation and its implications for second language acquisition. In A. F. Healy & L. E. Bourne (Eds.),
Foreign language learning: Psycholinguistic experiments on training and retention
(pp. 365–395). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kuhl, P., Williams, K. A., Lacerda, F., Stevens, K. N., & Lindblom, B. (1992). Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age.
Science, 255
, 606–608.
Landauer, T. K. (1975). Memory without organization: Properties of a model with random storage and undirected retrieval.
Cognitive Psychology, 7
, 495–531.
Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.),
Cerebral mechanisms in behavior
(pp. 112–131). New York, NY: Wiley.
Latash, M. L. (2008).
Neurophysiolgical basis of movement
(2nd ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Light, L., & Carter-Sobell, L. (1970). Effects of changed semantic context on recognition memory.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9
, 1–11.
Limb, C. J., & Braun, A. R. (2008). Neural substrates of spontaneous musical performance: An fMRI study of jazz improvisation.
PLoS ONE, 3
(2), e1679.
Lindsay, P. H., & Norman, D. A. (1977).
Human information processing: An introduction to psychology
(2nd ed.). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13
, 585–589.
Loftus, E. F. (1991). The glitter of everyday memory…and the gold.
American Psychologist, 46
, 16–18.
Loftus, E. R., & Loftus, G. R. (1980). On the permanence of stored information in the human brain.
American Psychologist, 35
, 409–420.
Logan, G. D. (1983). Time, information, and the various spans in typewriting. In W. E. Cooper (Ed.),
Cognitive aspects of skilled typewriting
(pp. 197–224). New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization.
Psychological Review, 95
, 492–527.
Logan, G. D. (2002). An instance theory of attention and memory.
Psychological Review, 109
, 376–400.
Logan, G. D., & Cowan, W. B. (1984). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of control.
Psychological Review, 91
, 295–327.
Lorenz, K. (1981).
The foundations of ethology
. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
Lötze, R. H. (1852).
Medicinische Psychologie oder Physiologie der Seele
. Leipzig: Weidmann’sche: Buchhandlung.
Lubow, R. E., & Gewirtz, J. C. (1995). Latent inhibition in humans: Data, theory, and implications for schizophrenia.
Psychological Bulletin, 117
, 87–103.
Luce, R. D. (1986).
Response times: Their role in inferring elementary mental organization
. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Luchins, A. S. (1942). Mechanization in problem solving.
Psychological Monographs, 54
(Whole No. 248).
MacDonald, M. C., Pearlmutter, N. J., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1994). The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution.
Psychological Review, 101
, 676–703.
MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review.
Psychological Bulletin, 109
, 163–203.
MacLeod, C. M. (1998). Directed forgetting. In J. Golding & C. M. MacLeod (Eds.),
Intentional forgetting: Interdisciplinary approaches
(pp. 1–58). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
MacWhinney, B., & Bates, E. (1989).
The cross-linguistic study of sentence processing
. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Mahon, B. Z., Costa, A., Peterson, R., Vargas, K. A., & Caramazza, A. (2007). Lexical selection is not by competition: A reinterpretation of semantic interference and facilitation effects in the picture-word interference paradigm.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 33
, 503–535.
Maier, N. R. (1931). Reasoning in humans. II. The solution of a problem and its appearance in consciousness.
Journal of Comparative Psychology, 12
, 181–194.
Mandler, G. (2011). From association to organization.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20
, 232–23.
Mandler, J. (2001). On the foundations of the semantic system. In E. M. Forde & G. Jumphreys (Eds.),
Category specificity in mind and brain
(pp. 315–374). East Sussex, U.K.: Psychology Press.
Marcus, G. (2008).
Kluge: The haphazard construction of the human mind
. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
Maren, S. (1999). Long-term potentiation in the amygdala: A mechanism for emotional learning and memory.
Trends in Neuroscience, 22
, 561–567.
Marks, L. E. (1975). On colored-hearing synesthesia: Cross-modal translations of sensory dimensions.
Psychological Bulletin, 82
, 303–331.
Marlsen-Wilson, W. (1973). Linguistic structure and speech shadowing at very short latencies.
Nature, 244
, 522–523.
Martin, D. (February 25, 2012). Ulric Neisser Is Dead at 83; Reshaped Study of the Mind.
New York Times
obituary.
http://www.nytimes.com/2012/02/26/us/ulric-neisser-who-reshaped-thinking-on-the-mind-dies-at-83.html?pagewanted=all
Mather, G. (2006).
Foundations of perception
. Sussex, England: Psychology Press.
Matin, E. (1974). Saccadic suppression.
Psychological Bulletin, 81
, 899–918.
Maynard Smith, G. (1982).
Evolution and the theory of games
. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Mayr, E., & Provine, E. B. (Eds). (1980).
The evolutionary synthesis
. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1: An account of basic findings.
Psychological Review, 88
, 375–407.
McClelland, J. L., Rumelhart, D. E., & the PDP Research Group. (1986).
Parallel distributed processing
. Volume 2:
Psychological and biological models
. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
McGaugh, J. L. (2000). Memory—A century of consolidation.
Science, 287
, 248–251.
McGeoch, J. A. (1932). Forgetting and the law of disuse.
Psychological Review, 39
, 352–370.
McGraw, M. B. (1943).
Neuro-muscular maturation of the infant
. New York, NY: Columbia University Press.
McGrayne, S. B. (2011).
The theory that would not die: How Bayes’ Rule cracked the enigma code, hunted down Russian submarines and emerged triumphant from two centuries of controversy
. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
McGurk, H., & MacDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices.
Nature, 264
, 746–748.
McKinstry, C., Dale, R., & Spivey, M. J. (2008). Action dynamics reveal parallel competition in decision making.
Psychological Science, 19
, 22–24.
McLeod, P., & Hume, M. (1994). Overlapping mental operations in serial performance with preview: Typing. A reply to Pashler.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A
, 193–199.
Mechsner, F., Kerzel, D., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). Perceptual basis of bimanual coordination.
Nature, 414
, 69–73.
Merzenich, M. M., Nelson, R. J., Kaas, J. H., Stryker, M. P., Zook, J. M., Cynader, M. S., & Schoppmann, A. (1987). Variability in hand surface representations in areas 3b and 1 in adult owl and squirrel monkeys.
Journal of Comparative Neurology, 258
, 281–296.
Meyer, D. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90
, 227–234.
Michel, J. B., Shen, Y. K., Aiden, A. P., Veres, A., Gray, M. K., The Google Books Team, Pickett, J. P., Hoiberg, D., Clancy, D., Norvig, P., Orwant, J., Pinker, S., Nowak, M. A., & Lieberman Aiden, E. (2011). Quantitative analysis of culture using millions of digitized books.
Science, 331
, 176–182.
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information.
Psychological Review, 63
, 81–97.
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960).
Plans and the structure of behavior
. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
Miller, G., Tybur, J. M., & Jordan, B. D. (2007). Ovulatory cycle effects on tip earnings by lap dancers: Economic evidence for human estrus?
Evolution and Human Behavior, 28
, 375–381.
Miller, J. O., & Ulrich, R. (2003). Simple reaction time and statistical facilitation: A parallel grains model.
Cognitive Psychology, 46
, 101–151.
Minsky, M. (1988).
The society of mind
. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.
Mitchell, M. (1996).
An introduction to genetic algorithms
. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Moles, A. A. (1966).
Information theory and esthetic perception
. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.
Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7
, 134–140.
Moorehead, A. (1969).
Darwin and the Beagle
. New York, NY: Harper & Row.
Moran, J., & Desimone, R. (1985). Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex.
Science, 229
, 782–784.
Moray, N. (1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11
, 58–60.
Morrison, C. M., & Ellis, A. W. (1995). Roles of word frequency and age of acquisition in word naming and lexical decision.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21
, 116–133.
Morrison, C. M., & Ellis, A. W. (2000). Real age of acquisition effects in word naming and lexical decision.
British Journal of Psychology, 91
, 167–180.
Morton, J., Crowder, R. G., & Prussin, H. A. (1971). Experiments with the stimulus suffix effect.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 91
, 169–190.
Munte, T. F., Altenbuller, E., & Janke, I. (2002). The musician’s brain as a model of neuroplasticity.
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3
, 473–478.
Nairne, J. S., Pandeirada, J. N., & Thompson, S. R. (2008). Adaptive memory: The comparative value of survival processing.
Psychological Science, 19
, 176–180.
Necker, L. A. (1832). Observations on some remarkable optical phaenomena seen in Switzerland, and on an optical phaenomenon which occurs on viewing a figure of a crystal or geometrical solid.
London and Edinburgh Philosophical Magazine and Journal of Science, 1
, 329–337.