How Animals Grieve (19 page)

Read How Animals Grieve Online

Authors: Barbara J. King

BOOK: How Animals Grieve
12.49Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub

Time
featured O’Barry’s recollection in a story about the film
The Cove
, named best documentary of 2009 at the Oscars. Directed by Louis Psihoyos, the movie tells the story of O’Barry’s activism against a heinous practice that occurs yearly in the small Japanese town of Taijii: the kill
ing
of thousands of dolphins, six months out of every twelve. (I haven’t been able to bring myself to watch
The Cove
, because in some scenes the camera is turned on dolphins who undergo slow, agonizing deaths.) This brutal practice, driven by the lucrative business of falsely marketing dolphin meat as whale meat, remained largely secret before
The Cove
hit it big. O’Barry stresses that the vast majority of Japanese people had been as unaware of it as everyone else. The cove’s location is secluded, and the dolphins’ killers were highly motivated to keep their activity quiet.

It was O’Barry’s history with dolphins that led him to animal activism and to his goal of exposing the Japanese dolphin slaughter to a wide public. Back in the 1960s, O’Barry had captured five dolphins from the wild and trained them to perform in
Flipper
. Once the five were installed in the Miami Seaquarium, O’Barry spent countless hours in their company. Once the show began to air, he and the dolphins watched it together on a television set brought right to the water’s edge, every Friday night at 7:30. That’s when O’Barry first realized that dolphins are self-aware: the dolphins—Kathy included—recognized themselves on the small screen.

In support of his claim that Kathy committed suicide in her tank, O’Barry points to the way dolphins breathe. For humans, breathing is an automatic process that requires no conscious thought. We breathe naturally, even while in deep sleep, and rarely think about our breathing during the day, except in special circumstances such as hard exercise or a moment of emotional upset. As I type at the computer right now, I am intently focused on choosing the right words to convey my ideas; I inhale and exhale without awareness of doing so. As “conscious breathers,” however, dolphins enjoy no such luxury; they must focus on the drawing of each breath. According to O’Barry, when a physically healthy dolphin chooses not to breathe, she intends to bring about her own death.

When twenty-six dolphins died off the coast of Cornwall, England, in summer 2008, one expert suggested suicide as a possible explanation. The dolphins beached themselves in four separate spots on a river in south Cornwall. When word of the stranding first got out, rescuers rushed to the scene and managed to save perhaps ten to fourteen others (in the immediate frenzy, no good census seems to have been taken). For reasons no one understands, the dolphins who died had ingested large amounts of mud; their lungs and stomach were simply full of the stuff.
Notably,
no fish were found in the dolphins’ stomachs, so the idea that the creatures stranded while foraging for fish was ruled out.

In reporting the mass death, England’s
Guardian
newspaper quoted a pathologist who examined the animals on behalf of the Zoological Society of London. Vic Simpson told the reporter, “On the face of it, it looks like some sort of mass suicide. We have seen strandings on beaches, sometimes with five to seven dolphins—but never on a scale like this.” O’Barry, then, isn’t a rogue voice in claiming the possibility of suicide by dolphins.

But what could be the dolphins’ motivation to strand? Unlike Kathy, kept in captivity by people in the entertainment industry, here were healthy animals (a fact confirmed by autopsy) swimming free in the wild. As it turns out, the British Royal Navy had been conducting sonar exercises in the area at the time of the dolphins’ death. The Ministry of Defence was quick to say that these exercises were too far away to upset the dolphins, but perhaps this remains an open question. Could the sonar have caused the dolphins to become confused and panic? Either way, the suicide hypothesis seems to me not to be ruled out by the sonar hypothesis. If dolphin biology was disrupted by the sonar to the degree that these animals felt significantly disoriented, might they have consciously chosen to beach themselves? Terrible events may cause animals (including humans) to fall into such an acutely emotional state that they behave in ways that lead to their deaths. The time course involved may be brief or prolonged. Flint comes to mind here, the grieving young chimpanzee who died so soon after his mother. We have seen that any number of animals, from apes to rabbits, may respond to emotional trauma by shutting down emotionally.

With examples like these—the dolphin Kathy, the Cornwall dolphins, the chimpanzee Flint—we veer into the tricky area of animal mental health. To start with, not every example of self-harm, even in humans, is rooted in the urge to die. Sometimes, depression leads to the inability to care for oneself or to eat or sleep properly, but this situation may exist separately from suicidal wishes. There may indeed be no link at all between outright self-injury and suicide: the American Psychiatric Association notes that the unfortunate trend of cutting among adolescent girls, though a form of self-injury, is not a suicidal behavior. In fact, most
mental
health professionals see people who cut into their flesh with blades or knives as striving to help themselves (though in a dysfunctional and dangerous way that signals a need for help), as the stab of physical hurt temporarily relieves their deeper emotional pain.

Nor is self-harm limited to humans. We see it also in captive chimpanzees—and not only chimpanzees who are subjected to repeated biomedical procedures in labs. Scientists Lucy Birkett and Nicholas Newton-Fisher collected twelve hundred hours of data on forty socially housed chimpanzees in six zoos in the United States and the United Kingdom. While much of the apes’ behavior was deemed normal, the abnormalities were sufficiently prevalent to be termed “endemic.” Suicide was not reported, but chimpanzees rocked repetitively, bit themselves, plucked their own hair, and ate feces. Some of these behaviors occurred at low levels and for brief periods, but it’s worth noting that every one of the forty zoo chimpanzees showed some sort of abnormality, while in 1,023 hours of focal animal sampling of wild chimpanzees in Uganda, not one of the abnormal behaviors was seen.

Given what we know of post-traumatic stress syndrome (PTSD) in wild elephants, however, I wouldn’t rule out the possibility of abnormal behaviors in wild chimpanzee populations in regions where apes come under threat from humans. When elephants’ early attachments within their families are disrupted because of poaching and war, the result is a breakdown of normal elephant behavior and culture. Gay Bradshaw and her coauthors (including long-term Amboseli elephant researchers) published a report to this effect in
Nature
. That elephants suffer from PTSD in elephant-killing zones emerges in part from their capacity to mourn their family members. With these elephants in mind, we can see that the zoo chimpanzees have turned their own wellspring of emotion back onto themselves: emotion felt in this case not for others’ lives lost and bonds disrupted but for a life drastically limited in physical, cognitive, and emotional ways.

In this thicket of connections among animal depression, self-harm, and suicide, two entwined lessons stand out. First, our species is part of the problem and needs to be part of the solution. Compassionate response saved some of the stranded dolphins at Cornwall and under-girds activists’ fight against the poaching of elephants for their ivory.
Compassionate
awareness leads to the realization that many animals now held captive—elephants, great apes, and dolphins among them—should be living, if not in protected reserves in the wild, at least in sanctuaries. Even well-intentioned zoos simply cannot provide for the psychological health of these creatures. Bile farms that imprison bears go off the charts in the harm they do to animals; no such place should be allowed to exist at all.

The second lesson involves animal grief: We humans don’t just study the phenomenon of animal grief. In a broad sense, we cause animal grief as well. We bring about conditions in the wild and captivity that lead animals to feel a sort of self-grief, and at times to feel empathy for others’ suffering. Whatever caused that mother bear on the Chinese bile farm to run into a wall, in the end, it was human behavior—human greed twinned with an insensitivity to animal suffering—that murdered her.

12

APE GRIEF

It’s November 22, 1968. Earlier that month, Richard Nixon had bested Hubert Humphrey in the US presidential election. A massive operation in Vietnam had initiated a sweep of the Ho Chi Minh Trail, resulting over time in the dropping of three million tons of bombs on Laos. Yale University began to admit women. And on this very day, the Beatles released their “White Album.”

In the forests of Tanzania, the echoes of these political and cultural events are faint. Here, the air rings with chimpanzee pant-hoots. In the Gombe Stream population, chimpanzees, some excitable, others calmer, are on their way to becoming household names for animal-behavior aficionados: Flo, Fifi, David Graybeard, Goliath. These chimpanzees, already in 1968, have been observed for eight years by Jane Goodall—who is no longer dismissed as a
National Geographic
cover girl now that her discoveries of tool use and hunting have rocked the scientific world.

On this morning, Gombe researchers Geza Teleki and Ruth Davis follow a group of chimpanzees who walk through dense undergrowth. Serious students of ape behavior, Teleki and Davis are engaged to be married. Neither is aware, of course, that before the end of the year, Davis will be dead. Goodall makes an emotional acknowledgment to Davis and the “long arduous hours” she spent at Gombe in her book
In the Shadow of Man
. “It may have been due to physical exhaustion,” Goodall writes, “that one day in 1968, Ruth fell from the edge of a precipice and was instantly killed. Her body was found only after a search of six days.”
Davis
was buried in Gombe. “Her grave is surrounded by the forest,” Goodall notes, “and reverberates, from time to time, with the calling of the chimpanzees as they pass by.”

What a terrible irony, then, that on this November morning, Teleki and Davis observe the immediate aftermath of a chimpanzee’s death from a fall. They arrive at a clearing where chimpanzees, as Teleki would write in a journal article five years later, “explode into frenzied activity and raucous calling, including shrieks, cries, screams, waa barks and wraaah calls.” In a dry stream bed within a gully, the chimpanzee Rix lies still. A necropsy will later determine that Rix’s neck had snapped, causing instant death. Teleki and Davis seemingly just missed witnessing what must have been his dramatic plunge from a fig or palm tree, which probably occurred while he was eating or resting.

Teleki’s article offers a blow-by-blow reconstruction of the events he and Davis observed between 8:38 a.m. and 12:16 p.m, which they recounted into a hand-held tape recorder for later transcription. Most striking in their account is the prolonged attention paid by sixteen chimpanzees to Rix’s body—and how that attention varies. The chimpanzees are highly aroused in the wake of Rix’s fall, but there’s no uniform expression of that arousal. Just as we saw in Cote d’Ivoire, where the chimpanzee Brutus served as gatekeeper, determining which apes were allowed to approach the dead female Tina, the Gombe apes’ personal ties and personality differences play a role in their responses to Rix’s body.

Death has come suddenly to the Gombe community, and the apes’ responses emerge as part of a fast-moving situation. In the little area around Rix’s sprawled and lifeless body, a wild mix of behaviors is playing out. “Aggressive, submissive, and reassurance actions are performed,” Teleki writes, “at high frequency and intensity by nearly everyone present, with many swift shifts in demeanor.” Let’s look at the males Hugo and Godi as a way of sampling some of the individual variation—and mercurial mood shifts—in the chimpanzees.

Hugo displays vigorously and, at one point, hurls several large stones toward the body, which do not hit Rix. Shortly thereafter, he stills himself (though his hair is still erected, a sign of arousal) and sits on a rock, where he’s joined by another male. Hugo gets up, stands right next to
the
body, and stares at it for several minutes. Next he resumes his high-energy display, running away from the corpse. Later, in the area around the body, he mates with a female. When, much later, the male Hugh walks away from the death site, other chimpanzees, including Hugo, follow, after one last intensive bout of peering at Rix.

Godi, an adolescent, responds somewhat differently. He vocalizes more persistently than Hugo, uttering
wraaah
calls. Coming near the body, he stares at it while whimpering and uttering other vocalizations. To Teleki, he appears “extremely agitated, more so than any of the others.” Throughout the next hours, Godi attends closely to the body. At 11:45, close to the time the group moves off, Godi is the only chimpanzee still watching Rix.

At first glance, it may seem that Hugo’s and Godi’s responses to Rix’s death differed only slightly. Both showed signs of arousal, and neither one—in fact, no chimpanzee at the scene—touched the body at any time during Teleki and Davis’s observations, toward the end of which the apes moved on. In this way, the Gombe chimpanzees, in their response to Rix’s death, differ quite a bit from the Tai chimpanzees, in their response to Tina’s death. At Tai, touch was an important element in the group’s reaction.

Teleki makes a point of noting, though, Godi’s “exceptional performance” that morning. Godi acted unlike the other chimpanzees in three ways: his proximity to the body, his level of agitation, and the frequency of his
wraaah
calling.
Wraaah
calls are “high-pitched, repetitious, plaintive wails,” Teleki writes, “which can carry for a mile and more along the acoustic funnels of steep valleys, convey[ing] an intense emotional state which cannot be adequately communicated in words.” Even though he didn’t touch the body, Godi was emotionally affected by what had happened to Rix. Godi, notably, had often accompanied Rix in their daily travels.

Other books

The Cats in the Doll Shop by Yona Zeldis McDonough
Seaside Hospital by Pauline Ash
The Heir of Night by Helen Lowe
Second Hope Cowboy by Rhonda Lee Carver
Frost on My Window by Angela Weaver
Mortal by Kim Richardson
SEAL Of My Heart by Sharon Hamilton