How Animals Grieve (15 page)

Read How Animals Grieve Online

Authors: Barbara J. King

BOOK: How Animals Grieve
4.03Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub

With the advent of herding, in Heinrich’s scheme, ravens’ association with death became tinged with doom. An anthropological view, though, suggests no simple linear chronology of hunting first and herding later, but instead a dynamic and overlapping set of subsistence behaviors that responded to environmental conditions. Given this fact, perhaps disparate cultural traditions may be a primary cause of the raven’s complex symbology. One sort of oral tradition regarding the raven may have emerged in one group, another sort in another location.

In their book
In the Company of Crows and Ravens
, John Marzluff and Tony Angell report a cornucopia of perspectives on corvids among Native American peoples. For Pacific Northwest native tribes, the raven may be seen as creator, clown, mischief maker, shape changer, or trickster. Some Native American groups relate a tale that explains the birds’ jet color. The Lakota Sioux say that crows were at first white. When a Lakota hunter masked as a buffalo caught the crow leader, he threw the crow into the campfire in reprisal for the warning it had issued to other animals about the impending hunt. The crow escaped but was blackened by the fire. For the Acoma Indians, the crow became black for a different fire-related reason: after the crow created the world, he rescued it from
fires;
dipping his wings in water to cool the burning earth, he turned black.

Underlying all this complexity is the fact that the corvids are incredibly smart and social. As a primatologist, I love that they are dubbed “the feathered apes” based on their cognitive and behavioral similarities to chimpanzees. Corvid calls are not just expressions of fear or arousal but communicate specific messages about predators, family members, and available resources. People, say Marzluff and Angell, can easily determine corvids’ emotional tenor by reading the “language” expressed through the varying arrangement of their contour feathers, an unusual communicative feature (and surely a key channel of communication among the birds themselves).

Corvids’ social groups are hotbeds of shared learning and communication, where individuals are recognized and problems are solved intelligently. Sometimes it seems the birds gather with the express intent of sharing information with each other. At the University of Washington, crows gather each morning at a certain parking lot next to the football stadium. Flock after flock arrives in successive landings. Marzluff and Angell note the defeaning cacophony of calls, and even they, expert ornithologists, wonder what’s going on. This parking-lot ritual has endured for forty years, with at least four generations of crows now involved. In the beginning, the location made sense in a practical way: a dump existed at the site, in which crows could scavenge for food. Now no food can be found nearby. The lot is not particularly warm, nor is it near the crows’ roosting site. Why do the birds still select this spot to gather? The crows are doing what their parents did, and their grandparents. It’s ritual now, a local tradition. The crows “regroup, catch up on the latest buzz, prepare for the day’s events, and shake the sleep out of their bones,” write Marzluff and Angell. It’s a cultural choice the crows make.

The popular theme in human myth and legend of crows’ and ravens’ close association with death, then, can be assessed from a scientific point of view against the backdrop of these birds’ social tendencies and intelligence. What we know about the feathered apes leads to a prediction that corvids may feel, and express, emotion when a flockmate dies. But
do
corvids really grieve? Marzluff and Angell report a curious thing that sometimes happens with crows: a loud, squawky cluster of birds,
numbering
in the hundreds or even thousands, gathers and stays together for about a quarter of an hour. Then there’s a period of silence, followed by a collective departure. Left behind is a single dead crow. What could that be about?

Crows tend to avoid places of danger, including locations where humans or other predators have captured flockmates in the past. If the single crow has died right in the midst of the group, perhaps the strange silence accompanies a process by which the survivors fix in their minds a place to avoid in the future. But could something more emotional be going on, like a crow funeral?

A controlled experiment might help with this question. Marzluff and Angell reasoned that if they were to place dead crows in a known study area, they might evoke the peculiar noise-silence-departure sequence from the resident crows. Testing that hypothesis, they found that they could not in fact evoke that behavior, but what did transpire was revealing. Within minutes of the carcasses’ appearance, resident birds uttered assembly vocalizations, which served to call in crows from the surrounding area. Soon, ten or more birds, all calling, circled over the dead ones. A few, residents of the study area, went to the ground for a closer look, perhaps checking to see if they knew the identity of the dead birds. A half hour later, it was all over. No silent period had ensued, and nothing that could be described as a funeral had taken place.

The parallels with the elephant research described in
chapter 5
are notable. In Kenya, Cynthia Moss reported behavior that suggested a preference among elephants for the bones of their dead relatives. Specifically, she saw a seven-year-old male caressing, for a longer period than anyone else in his group, the bones of his mother’s jaw. Such attention, if directed preferentially toward the bones of animals that were loved when alive, could be one measure of elephant mourning. Then Moss and two colleagues made the same choice that the corvid scientists did—to follow up on an impression by launching an experiment. It turned out that elephants did not prefer bones of their own matriarch to bones of other groups’ matriarchs.

Just as I couldn’t dismiss the meaning of Moss’s anecdotal observation of an emotional elephant youngster because of the experimental data, I can’t now dismiss the idea that something of emotional
significance
is going on with corvids’ response to death. Marzluff and Angell don’t dismiss it either. In their more recent book
Gifts of the Crow,
they devote a chapter to “passion, wrath, and grief” among corvids. There they recount what happened when a golf ball hit a crow on a Seattle golf course. The precision strike was an accident, of course; concerned golfers who witnessed the crow go down were startled to see that another crow immediately came to its aid. This second bird pulled on the first crow’s wings, calling out all the while. Five more crows soon arrived. At that point, three crows together “began pecking and pulling on the apparently dead bird, trying to lift it up by the wings.” The golfers assumed the bird would not survive and moved on, only to learn two holes later from other players that the stricken crow had in fact revived and flown off.

This anecdote sure looks like the expression of compassion for an injured flockmate. In some cases, though, corvids respond to an injured companion by killing it. And once in a while a crow mob will gang up and kill a crow who doesn’t even seem to be injured. These are complex birds, and there’s no predictable outcome of their social encounters. Still, the helping behavior described sets the stage for thinking about love and grief in corvids. Marzluff and Angell emphasize that crows and ravens “routinely” gather around bodies of their own dead. That response may be adaptive, they think, to the extent that it aids the birds in assessing what killed their dead confederate (thus increasing their chances of avoiding that fate themselves). It may, in addition, help the birds size up their new place in the flocks’ shifting hierarchy.

“We also suspect,” Marzluff and Angell write, “that mates and relatives mourn their loss.” Given that we’re talking about complex creatures—the feathered apes—I suspect it too.

9

SEA OF EMOTION

DOLPHINS, WHALES, AND TURTLES

In the Amvrakikos Gulf off the coast of Greece, a mother, alone in the water with her offspring, tried over and over again to revive her child. She lifted the small body above the surface, then pushed it down under the water, then repeated the cycle.

The mother was a bottlenose dolphin, and the calf was a dead newborn; the observers were scientists aboard a research vessel from the Tethys Institute. They found themselves watching “desperate behavior” on the part of the mother, who, over the course of two days in 2007, vocalized and touched her calf with her rostrum and pectoral fins. The mother seemed unable to accept the fact of her calf’s death. Other dolphins from the mother’s group, numbering about 150, occasionally approached to observe the drama, but they neither interfered nor lingered. It was just the mother, and the dead baby, together in the water.

The researchers felt concern for the mother, and with good reason. Over about four hours in the course of two days, they never saw her feed. Given dolphins’ high metabolic rates, her total focus on the calf could have put her health in danger. The baby, meanwhile, had already begun to decay. Compared to the babies of monkeys and apes, marine mammal infants undergo rapid decomposition after death. The mother already had begun to remove pieces of skin and tissue from the corpse.

A passage from the scientists’ report about this dolphin mother described their own compassion:

The
researchers on board did not feel like taking the calf away from the mother to perform scientific investigations (e.g., a necropsy of the calf). Their decision was intended as a form of respect towards a highly-evolved animal, the deep suffering of whom was obvious enough.

The researchers knew what they were seeing: maternal grief.

Just like monkey and ape mothers, dolphin mothers will sometimes carry the corpses of their dead infants. At times, though not in the Greek case, the mother’s social group attends acutely to this behavior. An early report of this type, from 1994, concerns bottlenose dolphins off the Texas coast. Some fishermen noticed that one dolphin, probably the mother, was laboring to keep a smaller deceased dolphin from washing ashore. Several adult dolphins moved in a clockwise direction around the pair; when the fishermen moved in closer, the animals began to slap the water with their tails. The maternal behavior continued for two hours and was observed again the next day (it was assumed that the same mother-infant pair was involved each time).

In a separate anecdote from the same report, volunteers from the Texas Marine Mammal Stranding Network observed an adult dolphin pushing a dead calf in the water. When the volunteers’ boat moved in, the adult swam away and resurfaced elsewhere, even as other dolphins swam nearby. By switching off the engine and drifting quietly closer, the rescuers managed to pluck the calf out of the water and bring it into the boat. At this, the mother became frantic, rushing underneath and at the boat.

I found myself wishing these volunteers had not intervened but instead had allowed the mother to experience her grief. The facts, though, temper this sentimental view: the mother could no longer help her baby, and she might have dangerously depleted her energy reserves had she kept on with such behavior. It’s even possible that this second Texas observation of maternal behavior involved the same dolphins as the first: it occurred only six days after, and twelve miles distant from, the earlier sighting. Had the marine-mammal volunteers spotted the same mother that the fisherman had? Could the mother possibly have endured in her efforts for that long a time? That the infant in the second sighting looked to be in worse physical shape than the first suggests that she may have.

In
2001 off the Canary Islands, rough-toothed dolphins were observed by scientists over the course of six days during whale-watching trips. One animal, presumably the mother, was seen pushing a dead newborn in ways that are by now familiar. This time, though, the mother had escorts—two adult dolphins who, Fabian Ritter reported in the journal
Marine Mammal Science
, “were swimming in a highly synchronous way, usually slightly in front of and constantly escorting the mother.” Other dolphins approached the mother and her escorts. The next day, similar behaviors were seen, and over the following four days, during four sightings, escorts were present three times. Meanwhile, the dead baby began to show signs of decay. By the fifth day, the mother left the calf alone for longer periods, but the dolphins still defended the corpse when a gull approached it. And, interestingly, the escorts began to participate in the calf-supporting behavior more directly: they swam, at times, with their backs underneath the body.

As is routine in the scientific literature, the published descriptions are dry, clinical, and stripped of emotion. But it may be significant for a profile of dolphin grief that adults other than the mother involved themselves with the infant’s body. Working from their database of individual photographs, scientists identified nineteen dolphins who participated in some way in the unfolding events. Of these, fifteen were, on the basis of previous observation, thought to be members of a single, tight social unit. Further, the dolphins’ travel speed was slower during this period than had ever before been observed. Over the six days, the dolphins moved very little from their original location. Ritter concludes that the group adjusted its behavior to the exceptional circumstances surrounding the calf’s death.

A tightly coordinated response among animals who communicated closely with each other, this event shows clearly that a dolphin social group may be affected by an infant’s death. It would be risky for me to assert that it shows as well the existence of collective dolphin grief. Yet the chain of logic isn’t weak: Given that mother dolphins exhibit grief and that dolphin pods are tight-knit social groups, it is entirely possible that dolphins other than the mother exhibit grief when a youngster dies.

Gregarious creatures, dolphins play exuberantly with partners in their own groups and sometimes also with whales. A wonderful series of
photographs
taken in Hawaiian waters shows play between a bottlenose dolphin and a humpback whale, once near Maui and another time near Kauai. In both cases, the dolphins draped themselves across the whales’ heads; the whales then reared up and the dolphins slid down their backs. At no time did the whales’ actions appear aggressive, and the dolphins’ cooperation over multiple “rides” was complete.

Other books

The Private Club by J. S. Cooper
Texas Wildcat by Lindsay McKenna
Blood Oath by Tunstall, Kit
Great mischief by Pinckney, Josephine, 1895-1957
Friends Upgrade by Stephanie Williams
The Secret Year by Jennifer R. Hubbard
Heart of the West by Penelope Williamson