Read Dog Sense Online

Authors: John Bradshaw

Dog Sense (47 page)

BOOK: Dog Sense
4.59Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

Indeed, if dogs are to continue to remain popular as pets, they need some focused selection for the specific qualities that make them rewarding companions; it is no longer enough to put these in third place, after external appearance and behavioral traits that reflect the original working role. Selection for such traits may be complex, but is achievable, as evidenced by the many dogs who (perhaps more by accident than design) comfortably fit into this niche today.

Prospective owners, however, often select dogs more by appearance than by personality. Perhaps if dogs were not so variable in appearance, thereby offering owners so much choice on the “outside,” more emphasis might be placed on the “inside.” Breed often determines how active a dog needs to be and, hence, whether that type of dog will suit the owner's lifestyle. But personality and the likelihood that a lasting bond with the owner will develop are much less influenced by genetics than by environment, so owners would do well to focus on whether the environments in which the dogs were raised have given them the best possible start for life as pets.

CHAPTER 11
Dogs and the Future

D
ogs have been man's best friend for thousands of years, and maybe that's why we take them so much for granted. They have shown themselves to be supremely versatile, carrying out a vast range of tasks in addition to being rewarding social companions. But do they have the capacity to continue reinventing themselves, as human society changes ever more rapidly? Yes, but they'll most likely need help along the way—help that canine science is ready to provide. Dogs and humans have gotten along pretty well so far without either completely understanding the other. As I see it, we humans—as the senior partners in this arrangement—must take primary responsibility for ensuring that the relationship continues effectively into the future. Indeed, we should be working to improve the understanding between humans and dogs, using the most up-to-date science, so that dogs can continue to live harmoniously alongside us.

Dogs provide mankind with many benefits. Not only do they continue to work for us in the old, traditional ways, but we are continually finding new roles for them—tasks for which their agility, intelligence, and ability to interact with the world are superior to our own. They also bring us the psychological benefits of companionship, providing relationships that complement those we have with members of our own species. Moreover, if we understand them properly, they can provide us with a fascinating glimpse into a different world, physically the same but perceived through different senses.

Canine science has brought us rich new insights into dog's uniqueness. Until about a hundred years ago, man's understanding of dogs was no more than a branch of folk psychology, a tradition built up over thousands of years of trial and error. Science began to intrude toward the end of the nineteenth century, on two fronts: Several of the early comparative psychologists, Thorndike and Pavlov among them, used dogs as convenient experimental animals, while Victorian naturalists and zookeepers provided the first comparisons with the wolf. A further burst of activity in the mid-twentieth century—an exchange of ideas among wolf biologists, canine geneticists, and the first veterinarians to take an interest in behavior—led to the first systematic understanding of the socialization process, yet also contributed a misinterpretation of wolf behavior that has dogged canine biology ever since.

Now, at the beginning of the twenty-first century, a new opportunity has arisen to integrate concepts pertaining to wolf and dog behavior, modern animal welfare science, and new knowledge about learning and cognition, thereby allowing us to update our understanding of dogs and how they would like us to care for them. As is always the case with science, this understanding will need constant revision in the light of new information; nevertheless, we know vastly more now than we did a hundred years ago about how to treat our canine companions. For one thing, dogs are canids but don't behave much like wolves. Superficial comparisons between any wild animal and the dog, which is perhaps the ultimate domesticated animal, are rarely helpful. Secondly, dogs have a unique capacity to form attachments to humans, and it is to people that their primary allegiances are directed. But it is only the capacity to form such attachments that is inborn: These have to be nurtured, primarily during the first three to four months of the dog's life. Thirdly, dogs' sense of smell is much more sensitive than ours and should be respected as such, not simply exploited.

These ideas are no-brainers; they are notions that any dog lover should find straightforward to accept, provided they have respect for the science that created them. As the scientific research becomes more widely disseminated through both academia and the mainstream media, they will be incorporated increasingly into the folk-psychology of dog-keeping.

Dog training is one area in which the new canine science has met with strong resistance, to the point where some trainers and self-proclaimed “behavior experts” have openly and deliberately attacked the credentials of those trying to disseminate reliable, science-based information. The idea that the majority of dogs are continually trying to take over control of the households in which they live is proving very slow to die. So indeed is the use of physical punishment: Shock collars, already widely available in the United States, are catching on in some quarters in the United Kingdom.

However, the situation is not simply one of punishment to prevent dominance versus rewards to encourage attachment. Indeed, some trainers decry the use of any kind of physical punishment, others see it only as a last resort, and still others consider it an essential and everyday part of the trainer's armory. Disagreements over methods have sometimes spilled over into personal attacks. Those who decry physical punishment portray it as unnecessarily cruel, sometimes going so far as to suggest baser motives in those who use such techniques, accusing them of cruelty for cruelty's sake.
1
On the other side, some trainers accuse their opponents of actively promoting bad behavior in dogs.
2

All this polarization and recrimination may obscure the fact that there are still several humane methods for training dogs, though their relative effectiveness has yet to be evaluated. Although there are probably as many ways of conceiving how to train a dog as there are dog trainers, there appear to be four specific facets of dogs about which trainers and behavior experts disagree most vehemently.

The first is whether or not to portray dogs as “pack animals.” Those supporting the “pack” notion often appear to use it as a wake-up call for owners who over-anthropomorphize their dogs, who treat them as little people. Since dogs are not little people and, indeed, can suffer if treated so, such reminders may be salutary if applied in moderation rather than to justify physical punishment.

The second is an ethical and philosophical dimension related to welfare and well-being. Some trainers place no emphasis on the question of whether physical punishment will cause suffering, rationalizing it as an experience that the dog, as a barely reconstructed wolf, will expect to receive as part of its upbringing. At the opposite extreme are those
trainers who abhor all forms of punishment, on the grounds that it will—by definition—cause suffering for the dog. Still others adopt a more moderate approach, maintaining that a certain amount of suffering can be balanced against the longer-term benefits that will accrue to the dog if it corrects its behavior. They regard physical punishment as a last-resort technique, justified only when the dog's long-term well-being would otherwise be threatened.
3
For example, some justify the use of shock collars to punish livestock-chasing, on the grounds that if dogs continued to engage in this behavior they would run the risk of being shot by a farmer, or euthanized. (Bear in mind that it is virtually impossible in any training regime to avoid all negative feelings in dogs; even just ignoring them when they're performing an unwanted type of attention-seeking is likely to make them anxious.) Where these trainers may disagree is in how to balance the trade-off: suffering now versus benefit later.

Thirdly, trainers make very different assumptions about dogs' cognitive abilities. Paradoxically, perhaps, trainers who work within the dominance framework have to presume an almost Machiavellian level of canine intelligence: The dog needs to be very smart indeed, smart enough to hoodwink its owner and thereby attain “dominant” status in the household. In the other camp, reward-based pet dog trainers rely on quite straightforward associative learning, both because it works and because it's easy for owners to understand how to implement it. Their methods tap into some of the most primitive parts of the dog's brain; they essentially sidestep the issue of how smart dogs may or may not be, relying on learning methods that evolved many millions of years ago. The point here is not that reward-based trainers think dogs are dumb but, rather, that simple training methods are easier to teach to owners than complex ones are. Guide-dog training, for example, while reward-based, makes much fuller use of dogs' cognitive abilities.
4
But, ideally, all training could take advantage of these abilities—including, for example, the dogs' capacity for social learning, which canine science is still in the process of revealing. Exploring such possibilities should be more profitable in the long run than pursuing ideas for which science has found no supporting evidence.

Fourthly, some trainers—especially those who train gundogs, sheep-dogs, and guard dogs—come from a tradition that portrays dogs primarily as tools. One method they advocate is to keep the dog in a kennel, away from what they view as excessive human influence. Others see the dog's natural place as an integral part of human society and insist that training should serve, above all, to reinforce that bond, even if the dog also fulfills a function. For example, security services in the UK are currently divided over whether patrol dogs can be kept at the handler's home or must be confined to kennels when off-duty. Given that such dogs need to be acutely attuned to people when they are working, it seems unlikely that this ability would be refined by long periods of isolation; moreover, no evidence has yet emerged that the kennel-housed dog is actually the more effective worker.

The differences between various schools of dog training are thus much more complex than the question of whether punishment is cruel or not, or whether or not trainers conceive of dogs as wolves. Their underlying philosophies and ethical standpoints are also different, so it is perhaps not surprising that they so often misunderstand, even misrepresent, one another. This is not helpful at a time when society expects dogs to be under the control of their owners to a greater extent than perhaps ever before.

Discipline in the sense of control, not discipline in the sense of punishment, is what is needed. Clearly a dog is not going to learn how to behave well simply and solely because it is loved, even though I suspect that is what many owners would like to happen—and may even
expect
to happen. Today, as I went jogging in the park near my house on the first sunny day of the year, I encountered nine dogs being walked off-leash by their owners. Only one out of the nine responded immediately when their owners wanted to recall them. The others all caused mild embarrassment for their owners, bouncing up at children, chasing cyclists, getting in the way of walkers, trying to scrounge food from picnickers, and so on. Assuming this unrepresentative sample is typical, such behavior must contribute to giving dogs a bad name.

I don't know what methods, if any, these owners had used to try to train their dogs to come back to them on command, but I'm willing
to bet that many had tried punishment. Certainly I saw several owners remonstrating with their dogs once they were able to wrench them away from whatever they had been doing. Although this may have been more for the benefit of the people their dogs had been annoying than for that of the dogs themselves, it is unlikely to have promoted the idea that coming back to their owners is a pleasant thing to do. It's more logical and straightforward to train dogs to come back to their owner because they want to than because they're scared not to, so there's clearly still a big gap between the ideal and the realities of dog ownership.

Proper understanding of training techniques and their diligent application are not only essential from a social perspective but also good for the dog-owner relationship. Moreover, it's been known for nearly two decades that attending training classes results in a more fulfilling dog-owner relationship.
5
Most dogs and their owners, whether they know it or not, desperately need easier access to better standards of dog training—but at present they are faced with a bewildering variety of claims and counterclaims from the various schools of trainer.

Unfortunately, there are no universally recognized standards for dog trainers.
6
The deep divisions between the various camps in the world of dog training, fueled by the rise of the Internet, have instead resulted in an explosion of “registers,” “associations,” “guilds,” and “institutes,” each claiming to be the last word in training and the treatment of behavioral disorders. Faced with a bewildering array of titles and their even more confusing acronyms, how are novice owners to choose a trainer who not only satisfies their training needs but also meets their ethical standards? Attempting to bring some clarity to this confusion is the Animal Behaviour and Training Council in the UK and at least three sets of dog-training guidelines in the United States: one contained in the mission statement of the Association of Pet Dog Trainers (APDT), one published by the Delta Society, and one disseminated by the American Humane Association. However, the plethora of organizations at both the state and national levels militates against the adoption of any universal set of standards. Effective self-regulation of the dog-training industry will be essential if we want to improve the lives of dogs (and their owners) in the twenty-first century.

BOOK: Dog Sense
4.59Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub
ads

Other books

The Killing Sea by Richard Lewis
Night Hungers by Kathi S Barton
Kijana by Jesse Martin
Rogue's Hollow by Jan Tilley
The Clancys of Queens by Tara Clancy
Asesinato en Mesopotamia by Agatha Christie
Conflict by Pedro Urvi
Out for Blood by Kristen Painter