Twilight Children (16 page)

Read Twilight Children Online

Authors: Torey Hayden

BOOK: Twilight Children
5.51Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub

“Cassandra, I’m not very happy right now, because I can’t understand what’s going on. You just wet your pants. About twenty minutes ago. And we went out onto the unit. We went to your bedroom and you changed your clothes and that’s why you’re dry now. We got the cleaning equipment and came back here to wipe it up. I’m not sure if you are confused about what happened or if you have forgotten what happened or if you are simply still angry with me and feel in the mood to argue. But it is hard for us to understand each other when we have these kinds of conversations.”

She started to cry then. They were not angry tears so much as despairing. “You don’t like me,” she sobbed. “No one likes me. It’s horrible here. You treat me terrible and I want to go home.”

To say I was confused at the end of that session was certainly an understatement. On one hand, Cassandra had a history of being very manipulative, sometimes pathologically so. It was fairly easy to interpret what had just happened as a deliberate effort to avoid responsibility, a calculated lie that threw the blame for her behavior onto me. Nor was it much of a stretch to interpret it as sociopathic behavior, that she was construing the events in an attempt to dominate me, that having assessed it was important to me to be understanding and accepting, she now sought to twist me into the dangerous and humiliating position of abuser.

But …

At that moment I couldn’t put my finger on “but.” Something felt seriously weird. Things just didn’t fit together in a way that made sense. Often when finishing sessions with children I would come away filled with unanswered questions, with the feeling that there was much more I had to understand before I’d know what to do. This, however, was a normal kind of not understanding, the kind where I knew it was me; I knew I simply needed more information, more time, or more insight. These instances were like doing a jigsaw puzzle where I didn’t know the picture that was being formed. I knew from the onset I didn’t know. And I knew likewise that it was unrealistic to expect to know until I’d found a sufficient number of pieces and fitted them into place. So that kind of not knowing was a normal state.

This was different. There was a bizarre, Alice-in-Wonderland quality to working with Cassandra. It felt as if every time I went to add another piece to her particular jigsaw puzzle, it changed into a completely different puzzle from the one I’d been adding pieces to before.

I didn’t have the time immediately after the session to think further on it, because I had a staff meeting and then needed to go out in the early afternoon to do two assessments.

It was after five when I returned to the unit, so Dave Menotti had already gone home. Not that I’d really had plans to consult with him over Cassandra so late in the day. Not that I’d even intended to do anything more with it myself at that point. However, as I came into the office, I unexpectedly saw the videotape of our session that morning sitting on my desk. In all the brouhaha over cleaning up after Cassandra had wet herself, I hadn’t gotten around to taking the videotape out of the machine in the observation room. Most likely someone else had wanted to record in the therapy room, and seeing my tape still in the machine, that person had brought it in and left it on my desk.

Impulsively, I popped it into the video recorder in the office and turned on the monitor, thinking I’d watch a few highlights to give me something to mull over during my commute home. I quickly got caught up in the tape, however; and instead of just whizzing through for highlights, I ended up watching it all.

Something odd caught my attention.

Just after Cassandra had wet herself, she said to me, “Babies come out in your wee.” And then, when I had used the word
urine
, she had replied, “I say ‘wee.’ Wee-wee. We, we have babies come out.”

At the end of the session, however, when Cassandra had denied responsibility for wetting herself, she said, “If that was pee on the floor, I’m going to tell my mom you made me touch it.”

Why would she use
wee
in one reference and
pee
in another? Linguistically, it is unusual for children to alternate slang terminology in that manner. They inevitably know plenty of words for bodily functions and as any adult around children experiences, they often glory in trying the full vocabulary out. This is for play or shock value, however, or for the sexual connotations the words carry. During genuine communicative conversation, however, people tend to stick to one preferred word or phrase and typically don’t vary.

It was possible that
wee
was the word used with her father and that this was also why it appeared to have additional sexual connotations, whereas
pee
might be her mother’s word, and thus, Cassandra was, indeed, expressing two different things with the words. Or perhaps she was accustomed to using two different expressions for the same act, because of the parents using different words. However, it still seemed odd to me that she would alternate so indifferently within the context of the conversation. I thought, it was almost like two different kids talking.

The penny dropped with that thought.

What if these
were
two different kids? As in multiple personalities. What if there was more than one Cassandra in there? What if she was telling the truth and the second girl genuinely hadn’t realized that first one wet her pants?

Chapter
16

G
erda was not proving any easier to crack than Cassandra or Drake. Each morning I stopped at the rehabilitation center before work to spend half an hour to forty-five minutes with her. However, she showed no inclination to speak to me spontaneously. Indeed, she showed little interest in speaking at all, as even direct questions produced only the barest of mutterings.

After a few days of wearisome struggles to make conversation, I resorted to reading to her, to demanding nothing from her, no words, no sound, indeed, no response. I just came and I read.

To say there was an actual method to my actions in doing this might have been stretching the term, but I did have a process in mind. My very, very earliest experience with elective mutism and, indeed, with special education itself, had occurred when I was still a college student and took on work as an aide in a preschool program for disadvantaged children. There I’d been given charge of a four-year-old named Mary who did not speak. She spent most of her time hiding under a piano, and I’d started our relationship by lying under there with her and carrying on a long, very one-sided monologue while she just watched me. When finally I ran out of conversation, I started reading to her. It took months to achieve a relationship with her and to get her speaking again, but it did happen and the connection between its happening and the long hours I spent apparently doing no more than reading to Mary was not lost on me.

In fact, during my years as a graduate student in special education I devised a small research project in learning disabilities on one occasion. This was in the 1970s when learning disabilities were the New Big Thing. Tests were popularly used to identify which of a child’s learning modalities were strongest and which were weakest as a way of explaining learning problems. For example, a person might be strong in visual memory and thus a good sight learner but might be weak in auditory discrimination and, thus, was poor at learning to read using phonics. All this kind of knowledge was in its infancy in that era and as seems a common human trait when first discovering things, we immediately think we know all about it. Thus, it became hugely fashionable to give these modality tests and then assume we knew precisely why a child wasn’t learning—because he or she suffered various visual, auditory, or kinetic weaknesses—and, therefore, all that was necessary to correct the problem would be to switch the child to his or her strengths. Which, of course, sounded really good in theory but isn’t quite how it worked out. But we didn’t know that then.

I was curious from the onset about more subtle factors that might be influencing successful outcomes. For my graduate project, I designed a small research study in which I divided the children with identified learning disabilities and poor reading performance into three groups. In the first group, the children were paired with a trained tutor who used the most up-to-date learning modality-based methods to help them with their reading; in the second group, the children were paired with an untrained college student who was instructed to read books, magazines, and comic books to them; and the third group was a control group who had no special interventions. The tutors/college students met their children twice a week for half an hour, and the project ran for six months. In the first month, both groups’ reading scores improved, although the improvement of the first group was statistically better. However, after six months, this wasn’t so. In both groups, the children had made statistically significant improvements in reading, and the first group, with the trained tutors, was still making slightly higher scores. However, it was no longer statistically significant. Both groups improved, whether the children were being actively taught or whether they were simply listening to an adult read.

My conclusion from this was not that we don’t need to actively teach children to read nor that we don’t need to address reading problems with specialist help but that the significant influence was human interaction, rather than the method used. To me the results of the study spoke of how much it matters to us that someone else is willing to take the time to be with us, that our problems tend to improve simply by being with people who pay positive attention to us.

Consequently, coming along each day to spend half an hour of time reading to Gerda not only didn’t seem like a waste to me, it seemed a feasible method of intervention.

I discovered, however, it didn’t to a man named Dr. Jamieson.

“And you are
who
, exactly?” he asked, as I came out of Gerda’s room Monday morning.

I gave my name and explained briefly that I was there because of a request from Joy Hansen, who had contacted me because of my experience in psychogenic language problems.

“Are you a relative?”

Realizing I must not have been clear, I said no again and proceeded to explain a second time that I had come at Joy Hansen’s request because of my experience in psychogenic language problems.

“You’re a child psychologist?” This was said in a tone of voice that left nothing to the imagination, and clearly here was not a man for ambiguity, so he added, “And so
what
exactly are you doing here?”

For a third time I explained I was here due to Joy Hansen’s asking my advice on Gerda Sharple.
Because
I had experience in psychogenic language problems.

“What’s that got to do with anything here?” he asked.

“Because,” I said, very slowly, very plainly, and in a tone of voice verging on what I’d use with a small, dull child, “Joy Hansen had questions regarding the nature of Gerda’s mutism.”

“She isn’t mute,” he replied and used the same very slow, very plain tone of voice back at me. “She’s had a massive hemiplegic stroke. Strokes cause brain damage. Brain damage affects the center of speech. That’s why she’s mute.”

Odd, I was thinking, how in such a short exchange we’d already decided we hated each other. Sad, too, as it wasn’t going to further Gerda’s case any.

“Is it all right that I come to see her?” I asked, because I sensed this was actually an issue of territory, not treatment methods.

“There’s nothing for you to see,” he replied. There was a small heartbeat of a pause and he stepped back slightly, his shoulders relaxing. “I mean, I appreciate what Mrs. Hansen’s trying to do, but she’s a social worker and that’s her job. I appreciate what you’re trying to do, too. Which is your job. Realistically, however, there’s not much you can do. This is a different part of the continuum than you’re accustomed to working with. At your end, everything is newborn. Just starting. Growth. At my end, it’s decay and death. So there’s a difference of approach to treatment that’s significant. Nothing is going to happen in this instance, no matter how much effort you put in. Mrs. Sharple has had a massive stroke. She’s eighty-two. She isn’t going to be the person she was, no matter who does what.”

“Is it all right that I come to see her?” I asked again.

He paused, regarding me. Then finally he nodded. “As you will.”

The encounter was depressing. Normally I see narrow-minded people simply as unable to think deeply, creatively, or persistently enough to realize the limits of their views, and this allows me either to ignore them or challenges me to show them alternatives. However, as I passed room after room of decrepit elderly patients, I had to acknowledge the reality in Dr. Jamieson’s dead-end thinking. We did come from opposite ends of the spectrum, and yes, my work was about growth. It was about potential, about opening up the possibilities of a lifetime ahead. I was used to thinking that way. I had come, challenged by the possibility of giving Gerda a chance to return to the cottonwoods and the cats that had defined her rural existence.

Was this realistic?

The stink of disinfectant, the moans, the incoherent “hellos” of an unseen Alzheimer’s patient followed me down the corridor. Who
was
I to be here? While I wanted to give Gerda a chance, only Pollyanna could deny how slim this chance was. I was no miracle worker. Not only did I not have any precedence in my own experience for successfully treating mutism in the elderly, but regardless of whatever progress Gerda and I might accomplish, decay and, ultimately, death
would
still be waiting in the wings anyway. That was just reality.

Other books

Midsummer's Eve by Philippa Carr
On Track for Treasure by Wendy McClure
Sex and Other Changes by David Nobbs
Night World 1 by L.J. Smith
The Coldest Night by Robert Olmstead
The Husband's Story by Norman Collins