The Illusion of Conscious Will (57 page)

Read The Illusion of Conscious Will Online

Authors: Daniel M. Wegner

Tags: #General, #Psychology, #Cognitive Psychology, #Philosophy, #Will, #Free Will & Determinism, #Free Will and Determinism

BOOK: The Illusion of Conscious Will
2.68Mb size Format: txt, pdf, ePub

How Things Seem

Sometimes how things seem is more important than what they are. This is true in theater, in art, in used car sales, in economics, and—it now turns out—in the scientific analysis of conscious will. The fact is, it seems to each of us that we have conscious will. It seems we have selves. It seems we have minds. It seems we are agents. It seems we cause what we do. Although it is sobering and ultimately accurate to call all this an illusion, it is a mistake to conclude that the illusory is trivial. On the contrary, the illusions piled atop apparent mental causation are the building blocks of human psychology and social life. It is only with the feeling of conscious will that we can begin to solve the problems of knowing who we are as individuals, of discerning what we can and cannot do, and of judging ourselves morally right or wrong for what we have done.

The illusion is sometimes fragile. There are days when we wonder whether we’re willing anything at all. In
The Reader
(1995), Bernard Schlink captures this feeling: “Often enough in my life I have done things I had not decided to do. Something—whatever that may be—goes into action; ‘it’ goes to the woman I don’t want to see anymore; ‘it’ makes the remark to the boss that costs me my head; ‘it’ keeps on smoking although I have decided to quit, and then quits smoking just when I’ve accepted the fact that I’m a smoker and always will be. I don’t mean to say that the thinking and reaching decisions have no influence on behavior. But behavior does not merely enact whatever has already been thought through and decided. It has its own sources . . .” (20).

But usually we assume that how things seem is how they are. We experience willing a walk in the park, winding a clock, or smiling at someone, and the feeling keeps our notion of ourselves as persons intact. Our sense of being a conscious agent who does things comes at a cost of being technically wrong all the time. The feeling of doing is how it seems, not what it is—but that is as it should be. All is well because the illusion makes us human. Albert Einstein (quoted in Home and Robinson 1995, 172) had a few words on this that make a good conclusion:

If the moon, in the act of completing its eternal way around the earth, were gifted with self-consciousness, it would feel thoroughly convinced that it was traveling its way of its own accord. . . . So would a Being, endowed with higher insight and more perfect intelligence, watching man and his doings, smile about man’s illusion that he was acting according to his own free will.

References

Abbey, A., and C. Melby. 1986. The effects of nonverbal cues on gender differences in perceptions of sexual intent.
Sex Roles
15: 283-298.

Abbott, K., and J. H. Flavell. 1996. Young children’s understanding of intention. Unpublished manuscript.

Abell, T. D. 1982.
Better felt than said: The holinesspentecostal experience in Southern Appalachia.
Waco, TX: Markham Press.

Acocella, J. 1999.
Creating hysteria: Women and multiple personality disorder.
San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Ajzen, I. 1985. From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. In
Action control: From cognition to behavior,
eds. J. Kuhl and J. Beckmann, 11-39. New York: Springer-Verlag.

———. 1991. The theory of planned behavior.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes
50: 179-211.

Aldridge-Morris, R. 1989.
Multiple personality: An exercise in deception.

Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Alexander, M. P., D. T. Stuss, and D. F. Benson. 1979. Capgras’ syndrome: A reduplicative phenomenon.
Neurology
29: 334-339.

Alloy, L. B., and L. Y. Abramson. 1979. Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser?
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
108: 441-485.

Alloy, L. B., and N. Tabachnik. 1984. Assessment of covariation by humans and animals: The joint influence of prior expectations and current situation information.
Psychological Review
91: 112-149.

American Law Institute. 1985. General provisions. In
Model penal code and commentaries,
212-268. Philadelphia: American Law Institute.

Angel, L. 1989.
How to build a conscious machine.
Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Anscombe, G. E. M. 1957.
Intention
. London: Blackwell.

Ansfield, M. E., and D. M. Wegner. 1996. The feeling of doing. In
The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior,
eds. P. M. Gollwitzer and J. A. Bargh, 482-506. New York: Guilford.

Ansfield, M. E., D. M. Wegner, and R. Bowser. 1996. Ironic effect of sleep urgency.
Behaviour Research and Therapy
34(7): 523-531.

Arnold, M. 1946. On the mechanism of suggestion and hypnosis.
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology
41: 107-128.

Aronson, E., C. Fried, and J. Stone. 1991. Overcoming denial and increasing the intention to use condoms through the induction of hypocrisy.
American Journal of Public Health
81: 1636-1638.

Asimov, I. 1970.
I, robot.
New York: Fawcett Crest.

Astington, J. W. 1991. Intention in the child’s theory of mind. In
Children’s theories of mind: Mental states and social understanding,
eds. D. Frye and C. Moore, 157-172. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

———. 1993.
The child’s discovery of mind
. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Astington, J. W., P. L. Harris, and D. R. Olson. 1988.
Developing theories of mind.
New York: Cambridge University Press.

Attneave, F. 1959. Comments: In defense of homunculi. In
Sensory communication: Contributions to the symposium on principles of sensory communication, July 19August 1,
ed. W. A. Rosenblith, 777-781. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Baars, B. J. 1988.
A cognitive theory of consciousness.
New York: Cambridge University Press.

Baddeley, A. C. 1986.
Working memory.
New York: Oxford University Press. Bair, J. H. 1901. Development of voluntary control.
Psychological Review
8: 474-510.

Baldwin, J. M., ed. 1902. Muscle reading. In
Dictionary of philosophy and psychology.
Vol. 2, 120-121. New York: Macmillan.

Bandura, A. 1977. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change.
Psychological Review
84: 191-215.

———. 1988. Human agency in social cognitive theory. Paper, 24th International Congress of Psychology, Sydney, Australia, August.

———. 1997a.
Selfefficacy: The exercise of control
. San Francisco: Freeman. ———. 1997b. Human agency: The emperor does have new clothes. Paper, CPA Conference, Toronto, 1-18.

Bandura, A., L. Reese, and N. E. Adams. 1982. Microanalysis of action and fear arousal as a function of differential levels of perceived self-efficacy.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
43: 5-21.

Banks, G., P. Short, A. J. Martinez, R. Latchaw, G. Ratcliff, and F. Boller. 1989. The alien hand syndrome: Clinical and postmortem findings.
Archives of Neurology
46: 456-459.

Banyai, E. I., and E. R. Hilgard. 1976. A comparison of active-alert hypnotic induction with traditional relaxation induction.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
85: 218-224.

Barber, T. X. 1969.
Hypnosis
. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.

Bargh, J. A. 1984. Automatic and conscious processing of social information. In
Handbook of social cognition,
eds. R. S. Wyer, Jr., and T. K. Srull. Vol. 3, 1-43. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

———. 1994. The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In
Handbook of social cognition,
2d ed., eds. R. S. Wyer, Jr., and T. K. Srull, 1-40. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

———. 1997. The automaticity of everyday life. In
Advances in social cognition,
ed. R. S. Wyer, Jr. Vol. 10, 1-62. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Bargh, J. A., S. Chaiken, P. Raymond, and C. Hymes. 1996. The automatic evaluation effect: Unconditionally automatic attitude activation with a pronunciation task.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
32: 185-210.

Bargh, J. A., and T. L. Chartrand. 1999. The unbearable automaticity of being.
American Psychologist
54: 462-479.

———. 2000. Studying the mind in the middle: A practical guide to priming and automaticity research. In
Handbook of research methods in social psychology,
eds. H. Reis and C. Judd, 253-285. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Bargh, J. A., M. Chen, andL. Burrows. 1996. Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
71: 230-244.

Bargh, J. A., and M. J. Ferguson. 2000. Beyond behaviorism: On the automaticity of higher mental processes.
Psychological Bulletin
126: 925-945.

Bargh, J. A., and F. Pratto. 1986. Individual construct accessibility and perceptual selection.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
22: 293-311.

Baron-Cohen, S. 1995.
Mindblindness.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Barresi, J., and C. Moore. 1996. Intentional relations and social understanding.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences
19: 107-154.

Barrett, D. L. 1994. Dreaming as a normal model for multiple personality disorder. In
Dissociation: Clinical and theoretical perspectives,
eds. S. J. Lynn and J. W. Rhue, 123-135. New York: Guilford.

Barrett, J. L., and F. C. Keil. 1996. Conceptualizing a nonnatural entity: Anthropomorphism in God concepts.
Cognitive Psychology
31: 219-247.

Barrett, W., and T. Besterman. 1926.
The divining rod: An experimental and psychological investigation.
London: Methuen.

Basedow, H. 1925.
The Australian Aboriginal
. Adelaide, Australia: Preece. Baskin, A. 1994. The spirit of Barbie.
Omni
76 (March).

Bass, E., and L. Davis. 1988.
The courage to heal: A guide for women survivors of child sexual abuse
. New York: Perennial Library.

Bateson, G., and D. D. Jackson. 1964. Some varieties of pathogenic organization. In
Disorders of communication,
ed. D. M. Rioch. Vol. 42, 270-283. New York: Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease.

Baumeister, R. F. 1997.
Evil: Inside human violence and cruelty.
San Francisco: Freeman.

———. 1998. The self. In
Handbook of social psychology,
4th ed., eds. D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, and G. Lindzey. Vol. 1, 680-740. Boston: McGraw-Hill.

Baumeister, R. F., T. F. Heatherton, and D. M. Tice. 1995.
Losing control
. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Baumeister, R. F., A. M. Stillwell, and T. F. Heatherton. 1994. Guilt: An interpersonal approach.
Psychological Bulletin
115: 243-267.

Beard, G. M. 1877. Physiology of mind reading.
Popular Science Monthly
10: 459-473.

———. 1882.
The study of trance, musclereading, and allied nervous phenomena.
New York.

Bechara, A., A. R. Damasio, H. Damasio, and S. Anderson. 1994. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex.
Cognition
50: 7-12.

Begg, I. M., R. K. Robertson, V. Gruppuso, A. Anas, and D. R. Needham. 1996. The illusory-knowledge effect.
Journal of Memory and Language
35: 410-433.

Bem, D. J. 1967. Self-perception: An alternative interpretation of cognitive dissonance phenomena.
Psychological Review
74: 183-200.

Other books

Proposition by Wegner, Ola
Tides of the Heart by Jean Stone
Ghostheart by Ananda Braxton-Smith
My Life: The Musical by Maryrose Wood
Body by Audrey Carlan
The Lure by Felice Picano
Obsession in Death by J. D. Robb