Read The Blackwell Companion to Sociology Online
Authors: Judith R Blau
resources. The basic argument in this work is that positive emotional experience is more likely when people occupy positive, high status identities, when they
have control over interactions in which they are embedded, and when they have
coping mechanisms available to them to handle any misfortunes that do occur.
As the sturctural symbolic interactionists would predict, those who occupy high
status, high power positions are more likely to experience pleasant, efficacious emotions as they maintain those identitites.
The Interplay of Affect and Instrumental Action
While the research on instrumental action and on emotional processes proceeded
without much cross-fertilization for a long time, several recent research projects look at how the two interact. The convergence of interests began with a theory
of emotions based on social exchange principles. Kemper (1978) proposed a
theory based on two dimensions of relationships that he argued were universal:
status and power. Relative positions on these two dimensions defined the key
aspects of a relationship and determined its emotional character (what Kemper
called structural emotions). Changes in status or power and attributions about
who was responsible for causing those changes led to specific emotions. For
example, status loss led to anger if the other person was responsible. The anger then motivated action to regain status. Status loss by another, if caused by
oneself, led to guilt.
Since Kemper's theory, several research streams have developed to examine
how instrumental exchanges affect emotional outcomes. The oldest and most
developed stream focuses on how perceptions of justice, equity, or fairness
develop from exchange interactions (see review in chapter 10 of Cook et al.,
1995). Indeed, this was a central concern of the original exchange theorists,
Homans, Blau, and Emerson. Not surprisingly, people feel they have been
unjustly treated and express anger when their rewards are lower than their
investments. Past reward experiences, status structures, power structures, and
reference groups all serve to complicate the process, however. People quickly
acclimate to any given levels of rewards (or a stable trajectory, like steadily rising rewards) and experience outcomes that fall below that expected level with a
sense of distressing loss (Molm, 1997). Closely related to expectation states
theory, status value theory argues that people generally expect congruence
between status value within a group and the level of rewards that one receives.
Therefore, people perceive fairness and are satisfied with their outcomes when
they receive what people like them generally get.
418
Lynn Smith-Lovin
Power is related to perceptions of fairness in a somewhat more complicated
way. Power use (and its lack of reciprocity) lead to perceptions of unfairness, but coercive power use causes much more negative emotion than reward withholding (Molm, 1997, pp. 190±218). Even when the two types of power use result in
equivalent departures from reciprocity, coercive power is seen as more nasty and intentional. These negative feelings by the low power person lead to behavioral
resistance ± reward withholding and even retaliation. This reaction, in turn,
makes the high power user lose part of the ability to reward; since loss is
experienced very sharply, the retaliation generally helps to mute coercive power use below what would be expected in the absence of the emotional response.
A more recent research thread in social exchange deals with the related
question of how trust and affective commitment (positive feeling) build up in
exchange relationships. Edward Lawler and his colleagues have posited a model
through which repeated exchanges each create small amounts of positive emo-
tion, cumulating over time to create a positive attitude toward the exchange
partner and behavioral commitment (high levels of exchange) to the ex-
change relationship (Lawler and Yoon, 1998). One of the most interesting
elements of this work is its analysis of how networks evolve over time in
response to emotional outcomes. In systems with some equal and some unequal
relations, pockets of positive, cohesive, committed relations tend to form among the power-equals, cutting them off from the more powerful actors within the
system. Since the more powerful actors generally have more potential exchange
partners, they spread their interactions among a large number of alters and
develop committed relationships with very few. On the other hand, when some-
what lower power, but power-equal, actors can exchange more frequently with
each other, they form highly cohesive subgroups.
Other network theorists have concentrated on the link between uncertainty,
behavioral commitment and trust. Basically, these researchers show that trust
can only develop when an exchange is risky (i.e. the situation allows for
untrustworthy behavior) and the trading partner acts in a reliable, trustworthy
manner. Kollock (1994), for example, showed that trust and behavior commit-
ment develops more quickly when the quality of the traded good is difficult to
determine (and therefore the opportunity to deceive is present). Molm (2000b)
showed that trust and affective commitment developed to highest levels in
reciprocal exchange (where partners did not negotiate to binding agreements),
especially when the power structure made behavioral commitment advantageous
for both.
Just as exchange researchers have shown how emotions are embedded in
exchange structures, expectation states researchers have begun looking at how
identity meanings and emotions can shape status processes. When status hier-
archies form in small, task-oriented groups, the high status people often experience more positive emotions, since they are encouraged to make contributions
and their contributions are more often marked by positive evaluation. Conver-
sely, the lower status people often feel negative emotions about being ignored or having their contributions commented on negatively. Lovaglia and Houser
(1999) demonstrated that these emotions tend to mute the status structure.
Social Psychology
419
The lower status group members are resistant to influence because of their
negative emotion, while the higher status members may be unusually accepting
because of their good feelings. Exchange researchers have noted the same thing
in the interaction of power, influence, and emotion: the negative emotion felt by low power people makes them much less likely to accept influence from high
power people for whom they would normally hold high expectations (Willer et
al., 1997).
The group processes researchers are also beginning to explore how identity
interacts with status and power processes. Identity that links low and high power positions seems to mute power use in exchange networks (Lawler and Yoon,
1998). If the subject in an expectation states experiment shares an identity with a simulated actor, that piece of positive identity information seems to combine just like other status information to form performance expectations (Kalkhoff and
Barnum, 2000).
Conclusions about the State of Sociological Social
Psychology
Psychology
The research strategy chosen by the major theoretical programs in sociological
social psychology has paid off handsomely. Isolating power, status, and identity processes so that they could be studied in pure form has led to a dramatic growth of theoretical knowledge in the past 25 years. Indeed, we have progressed so far that researchers are starting to put the picture back together again. In just the past five years, several studies have appeared that examine how status, power,
identity, and emotion interact in more complex situations. Our increasing under-
standing of these interactions has also increased the interplay between experi-
mental, survey, and ethnographic researchers. As the experimentalists, who
primarily test theories, develop more complex views of how basic processes
interact, their theories become more useful to survey and ethnographic research-
ers, who necessarily deal with a more complex social situation.
It is, of course, more difficult to say where we are going than to say where we
have been. One expects the current trend toward studying the interactions
between status, power, identity, and emotion to continue. But advances will
certainly come within the faces of social psychology as well.
In social exchange, people are beginning to think about how exchange net-
works change. Studies of network dynamics ± how networks evolve through
different power structures ± will constitute much future work. Since the research has focused so heavily on networks where exchange in one relation is negatively
correlated with exchange in another, the future might lie in the study of posi-
tively connected networks ± those where different resources are obtained from
alters or where resources must be passed through an intermediary to obtain
rewards. And ultimately, of course, we will have to study how complex exchange
networks that mix the different types of connections operate.
In the study of status processes, theorists are beginning to consider what
happens when you relax the basic scope conditions under which the theory
420
Lynn Smith-Lovin
should operate: the collectively oriented, goal seeking group. The exchange-
based logic of the theory depends on these scope conditions (they are the only
reason why we would expect an actor to defer to a higher expectation alter in
order to obtain the collective reward). So the puzzle is why so many of the
expectation states processes seem to operate outside the range of these situa-
tions. Researchers will explore a variety of avenues before understanding the
phenomenon, but one suspects that issues of identity will play a major role in the answer.
The study of identity and emotion must focus to some degree on exploring the
differences between the Heise and Burke models that now dominant the field.
There also may be a return to the more network-ecological view of the self that
characterized the structural symbolic interactionists' early efforts (Stryker,
1980). Once we understand how the social actor attempts to maintain identity
meanings through social interaction and emotion display, we can return to the
central question of how those identities and their meanings are obtained, and
what evokes them in one situation as opposed to another.
The research on emotion management and emotion norms has been largely
descriptive until now. Now that the existence of such norms (and their variation across historical time and across societies) is well established, we need additional work to establish the sources of the norms. What explains cultural variations in normative structure? How do emotion norms and other aspects of the social
fabric co-evolve over history? In addition, Hochschild's (1983) original work
suggested a number of hypotheses that we need to explore. She suggested that
the class structure influences emotional socialization, so that middle-class children learn skills that better suit them for emotional labor. She also argued that participation in emotional labor over a long period tends to alienate workers
from their authentic feelings, creating mental health problems. Data about
emotional experience over many occupations and class backgrounds will be
necessary to test such hypotheses.
Part VIII
Social Action
29
Immigrant Women and Paid
Domestic Work: Research, Theory,
and Activism
Pierrette Hondagneu-Sotelo
Many commentators refer to paid domestic work as thèìnvisible occupation.''
The work occurs in private households; it is generally performed in isolation,
without the company of co-workers or managers; and in the United States it has
historically been the province of marginalized women, of women of color and of
immigrant women. The occupation did achieve national visibility for a moment
in 1993 with the revelation that two female nominees for attorney general had
hired undocumented immigrant workers to care for their children in their
homes. Yet even as national attention focused on nannies as domestic workers,
the objections raised by the Senate inquisitors, the media, and the constituents centered on thèìllegality'' of hiring unauthorized immigrant workers and in
particular on Zoe Baird's failure to pay the requisite taxes and make social
security payments. This focus obscured issues that have to do with basic work
rights of domestic workers. In fact, the media attention ignored the voices and
concerns of the domestic workers themselves.
Who performs paid domestic work in the United States today? While domestic
employees are a diverse group that include European au pairs, college students,
and laid-off aerospace workers, the principal entrants into the occupation are
Latina and Caribbean immigrant women. They represent a group of workers
who, due to their class, race, gender, and legal status, are among the most
disenfranchised and vulnerable in our society. Little wonder, then, that their
This chapter was originally published in Heidi Gottfried (ed.), Feminism and Social Change: Bridging Theory and Practice (Champaign, IL: University of Illinois Press, 1996), copyright # Board of Trustees of the University of Illinois 1996. Reproduced by kind permission of the Board of Trustees of the University of Illinois.